Co‐option and the evolution of food sharing in vampire bats

Ethology ◽  
2021 ◽  
Vol 127 (10) ◽  
pp. 837-849 ◽  
Author(s):  
Gerald G. Carter
Keyword(s):  
2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Simon P. Ripperger ◽  
Gerald G. Carter

AbstractStable social bonds in group-living animals can provide greater access to food. A striking example is that female vampire bats often regurgitate blood to socially bonded kin and nonkin that failed in their nightly hunt. Food-sharing relationships form via preferred associations and social grooming within roosts. However, it remains unclear whether these cooperative relationships extend beyond the roost. To evaluate if long-term cooperative relationships in vampire bats play a role in foraging, we tested if foraging encounters measured by proximity sensors could be explained by wild roosting proximity, kinship, or rates of co-feeding, social grooming, and food sharing during 22 months in captivity. We assessed evidence for six hypothetical scenarios of social foraging, ranging from individual to collective hunting. We found that female vampire bats departed their roost individually, but often re-united far outside the roost. Nonrandomly repeating foraging encounters were predicted by within-roost association and histories of cooperation in captivity, even when controlling for kinship. Foraging bats demonstrated both affiliative and competitive interactions and a previously undescribed call type. We suggest that social foraging could have implications for social evolution if ‘local’ cooperation within the roost and ‘global’ competition outside the roost enhances fitness interdependence between frequent roostmates.


2020 ◽  
Vol 30 (7) ◽  
pp. 1275-1279.e3 ◽  
Author(s):  
Gerald G. Carter ◽  
Damien R. Farine ◽  
Rachel J. Crisp ◽  
Julia K. Vrtilek ◽  
Simon P. Ripperger ◽  
...  
Keyword(s):  

2021 ◽  
Vol 8 (7) ◽  
pp. 210266
Author(s):  
Rachel J. Crisp ◽  
Lauren J. N. Brent ◽  
Gerald G. Carter

When group-living animals develop individualized social relationships, they often regulate cooperation and conflict through a dominance hierarchy. Female common vampire bats have been an experimental system for studying cooperative relationships, yet surprisingly little is known about female conflict. Here, we recorded the outcomes of 1023 competitive interactions over food provided ad libitum in a captive colony of 33 vampire bats (24 adult females and their young). We found a weakly linear dominance hierarchy using three common metrics (Landau's h ’ measure of linearity, triangle transitivity and directional consistency). However, patterns of female dominance were less structured than in many other group-living mammals. Female social rank was not clearly predicted by body size, age, nor reproductive status, and competitive interactions were not correlated with kinship, grooming nor food sharing. We therefore found no evidence that females groomed or shared food up a hierarchy or that differences in rank explained asymmetries in grooming or food sharing. A possible explanation for such apparently egalitarian relationships among female vampire bats is the scale of competition. Female vampire bats that are frequent roostmates might not often directly compete for food in the wild.


2006 ◽  
Vol 14 (3) ◽  
pp. 223-238 ◽  
Author(s):  
Mario Paolucci ◽  
Rosaria Conte ◽  
Gennaro Di Tosto

2017 ◽  
Vol 28 (2) ◽  
pp. 565-569 ◽  
Author(s):  
Gerald G. Carter ◽  
Gerald S. Wilkinson ◽  
Rachel A. Page

Ethology ◽  
2011 ◽  
Vol 118 (3) ◽  
pp. 260-268 ◽  
Author(s):  
Christian C. Voigt ◽  
Silke L. Voigt-Heucke ◽  
Karin Schneeberger

2013 ◽  
Vol 280 (1753) ◽  
pp. 20122573 ◽  
Author(s):  
Gerald G. Carter ◽  
Gerald S. Wilkinson

Common vampire bats often regurgitate food to roost-mates that fail to feed. The original explanation for this costly helping behaviour invoked both direct and indirect fitness benefits. Several authors have since suggested that food sharing is maintained solely by indirect fitness because non-kin food sharing could have resulted from kin recognition errors, indiscriminate altruism within groups, or harassment. To test these alternatives, we examined predictors of food-sharing decisions under controlled conditions of mixed relatedness and equal familiarity. Over a 2 year period, we individually fasted 20 vampire bats ( Desmodus rotundus ) and induced food sharing on 48 days. Surprisingly, donors initiated food sharing more often than recipients, which is inconsistent with harassment. Food received was the best predictor of food given across dyads, and 8.5 times more important than relatedness. Sixty-four per cent of sharing dyads were unrelated, approaching the 67 per cent expected if nepotism was absent. Consistent with social bonding, the food-sharing network was consistent and correlated with mutual allogrooming. Together with past work, these findings support the hypothesis that food sharing in vampire bats provides mutual direct fitness benefits, and is not explained solely by kin selection or harassment.


2015 ◽  
Vol 282 (1819) ◽  
pp. 20152524 ◽  
Author(s):  
Gerald G. Carter ◽  
Gerald S. Wilkinson

Regurgitations of blood among vampire bats appear to benefit both direct and indirect fitness. To maximize inclusive fitness, reciprocal food sharing should occur among close kin. Why then do females with kin roost-mates help non-kin? We tested the hypothesis that helping non-kin increases a bat's success at obtaining future donations by expanding its network of potential donors. On six occasions, we individually fasted 14 adult females and measured donations from 28 possible donors. Each female was fasted before, during and after a treatment period, when we prevented donations from past donors (including 10 close relatives) by simultaneously fasting or removing them. This experiment was designed to detect partner switching and yielded three main results. First, females received less food when we prevented donations from a past donor versus a control bat. Donors within a group are therefore not interchangeable. Second, the treatment increased the variance in donors' contributions to food received by subjects, suggesting the possibility of alternative responses to a partner's inability to reciprocate. Finally, bats that fed more non-kin in previous years had more donors and received more food during the treatment. These results indicate that a bat can expand its network of possible donors by helping non-kin.


2013 ◽  
Vol 6 (6) ◽  
pp. e25783 ◽  
Author(s):  
Gerald Carter ◽  
Gerald Wilkinson
Keyword(s):  

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document