Early Hominids

2018 ◽  
pp. 255-298
Author(s):  
Russell L. Ciochan
Keyword(s):  
1991 ◽  
Vol 334 (1270) ◽  
pp. 211-221 ◽  

New evidence for the tissue types exploited by early hominids from carcasses possibly acquired through scavenging is derived from the larger mammal bone assemblages from FLK I, level 22 ( Zinjanthropus floor), and FLKN levels 1 and 2 from Bed I, Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania. Published skeletal part profiles from the two archaeological sites are evaluated using (i) modern observations on the sequence by which carnivores consume carcass parts in order to assess the timing of hominid access to carcasses, and (ii) measurements of flesh and marrow yields to assess the tissue types sought and acquired. These results suggest that the maximization of marrow (fat) yields, not flesh (protein) yields, was the criterion shaping decisions about carcass processing. Because of evidence for density-dependent destruction of some flesh-bearing parts by scavengers of the hominid-butchered assemblages, however, it is uncertain whether carcass parts were transported and acquired by hominids in a largely defleshed condition. The results on tissue types acquired are combined with a discussion of predation risk, feeding competition and the equipment needs of carcass processing in an attempt to identify archaeological test implications of competing hypotheses for the socio-economic function of the earliest archaeological sites.


Nature ◽  
1987 ◽  
Vol 328 (6132) ◽  
pp. 673-674 ◽  
Author(s):  
ALAN E. MANN ◽  
MICHELLE LAMPL ◽  
JANET MONGE
Keyword(s):  

Science ◽  
1975 ◽  
Vol 189 (4196) ◽  
pp. 63-63
Author(s):  
Milford H. Wolpoff ◽  
C. Loring. Brace
Keyword(s):  

Nature ◽  
1970 ◽  
Vol 227 (5259) ◽  
pp. 747-748 ◽  
Author(s):  
DAVID PILBEAM
Keyword(s):  

2006 ◽  
Vol 29 (3) ◽  
pp. 211-224 ◽  
Author(s):  
Victor Nell

Cruelty is the deliberate infliction of physical or psychological pain on other living creatures, sometimes indifferently, but often with delight. Though cruelty is an overwhelming presence in the world, there is no neurobiological or psychological explanation for its ubiquity and reward value. This target article attempts to provide such explanations by describing three stages in the development of cruelty. Stage 1 is the development of the predatory adaptation from the Palaeozoic to the ethology of predation in canids, felids, and primates. Stage 2, through palaeontological and anthropological evidence, traces the emergence of the hunting adaptation in the Pliocene, its development in early hominids, and its emotional loading in surviving forager societies. This adaptation provides an explanation for the powerful emotions – high arousal and strong affect – evoked by the pain-blood-death complex. Stage 3 is the emergence of cruelty about 1.5 million years ago as a hominid behavioural repertoire that promoted fitness through the maintenance of personal and social power. The resulting cultural elaborations of cruelty in war, in sacrificial rites, and as entertainment are examined to show the historical and cross-cultural stability of the uses of cruelty for punishment, amusement, and social control.Effective violence prevention must begin with perpetrators, not victims. If the upstream approaches to violence prevention advocated by the public-health model are to be effective, psychologists must be able to provide violence prevention workers with a fine-grained understanding of perpetrator gratifications. This is a distasteful task that will compel researchers to interact with torturers and abusers, and to acknowledge that their gratifications are rooted in a common human past. It is nonetheless an essential step in developing effective strategies for the primary prevention of violence.


Nature ◽  
1987 ◽  
Vol 326 (6112) ◽  
pp. 493-496 ◽  
Author(s):  
A. D. Beynon ◽  
B. A. Wood
Keyword(s):  

2000 ◽  
Vol 27 (8) ◽  
pp. 655-663 ◽  
Author(s):  
Shannon Patrick McPherron
Keyword(s):  

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document