Selective Associations and Adaptive Specializations: Taste Aversions and Phobias

2019 ◽  
pp. 145-180
Author(s):  
Vincent M. LoLordo ◽  
Anastasia Droungas
1971 ◽  
Vol 78 (6) ◽  
pp. 459-486 ◽  
Author(s):  
Paul Rozin ◽  
James W. Kalat

Ethology ◽  
2008 ◽  
Vol 114 (7) ◽  
pp. 633-645 ◽  
Author(s):  
Anders Brodin ◽  
Johan J. Bolhuis

Behaviour ◽  
1990 ◽  
Vol 112 (1-2) ◽  
pp. 84-98 ◽  
Author(s):  
Verena Dasser ◽  
Paul Hoyningen-Huene ◽  
Hans Kummer

AbstractThe paper expresses the authors' views on the growing interest in primate social cognition, particularly among descriptive primate ethologists. Its characteristics are the hope to extract cognitive interpretations from field anecdotes, the free use of intentional language, and the untested and so far untestable idea that primate intelligence was selected in social contexts. We believe that 1) To understand how the animal itself represents the structure of its group or its habitat is perhaps the most ethological ethology there is and well worth pursuing. The study of social cognition, in particular, has long been neglected. 2) However, it requires of ethologists that they learn from established cognitive science and integrate its categories with their own. This is an interdisciplinary enterprise. 3) A traditional inductive study begins with anecdotes, which then are translated into hypotheses, which in turn are subjected to empirical tests including experiments. Sociobiology began to publish hypotheses without tests; the social cognition move now goes on to publish anecdotes without hypotheses, with a strong penchant for anthropomorphic interpretations in terms of social manipulation. This is little more than applying human prejudice. Phylogenetic and cognitive insights will come from testing alternative levels of organization in an animal's social knowledge about the same behavioral interaction. The experiment is the largely unavoidable method. Examples are given. 4) The speculation of the social origin of primate intelligence is tentatively interpreted in two possible directions. A version based on ROZIN's (1976) view that generalized mammal intelligence evolved from context-specific "Adaptive Specializations" seems the more accessible to ethological thinking and method.


2007 ◽  
Vol 30 (4) ◽  
pp. 420-421 ◽  
Author(s):  
Simon M. Reader

AbstractBehavioral innovations induced by the social or physical environment are likely to be of great functional and evolutionary importance, and thus warrant serious attention. Innovation provides a process by which animals can adjust to changed environments. Despite this apparent adaptive advantage, it is not known whether innovative propensities are adaptive specializations. Furthermore, the varied psychological processes underlying innovation remain poorly understood.


Author(s):  
Changjun Peng ◽  
Jin-Long Ren ◽  
Cao Deng ◽  
Dechun Jiang ◽  
Jichao Wang ◽  
...  

Abstract The transition of terrestrial snakes to marine life approximately 10 million years ago (Ma) is ideal for exploring adaptive evolution. Sea snakes possess phenotype specializations including laterally compressed bodies, paddle-shaped tails, valvular nostrils, cutaneous respiration, elongated lungs and salt glands yet knowledge on the genetic underpinnings of the transition remain limited. Herein, we report the first genome of Shaw’s sea snake (Hydrophis curtus) and use it to investigate sea snake secondary marine adaptation. A hybrid assembly strategy obtains a high quality genome. Gene family analyses date a pulsed coding-gene expansion to about 20 Ma, and these genes associate strongly with adaptations to marine environments. Analyses of selection pressure and convergent evolution discover the rapid evolution of protein-coding genes, and some convergent features. Additionally, 108 conserved non-coding elements appear to have evolved quickly, and these may underpin the phenotypic changes. Transposon elements may contribute to adaptive specializations by inserting into genomic regions around functionally related coding genes. The integration of genomic and transcriptomic analyses indicates independent origins and different components in sea snake and terrestrial snake venom; the venom gland of the sea snake harbours the highest PLA2 (17.23%) expression in selected elapids and these genes may organize tandemly in the genome. These analyses provide insights into the genetic mechanisms that underlay the secondary adaptation to marine and venom production of this sea snake.


2011 ◽  
Vol 82 (5) ◽  
pp. 945-956 ◽  
Author(s):  
I. Teschke ◽  
E.A. Cartmill ◽  
S. Stankewitz ◽  
S. Tebbich

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document