Testing the cost of reproduction with the rotifer Brachionus patulus at different pH levels

Limnology ◽  
2010 ◽  
Vol 12 (2) ◽  
pp. 145-149 ◽  
Author(s):  
Xu Wang Yin ◽  
Cui Juan Niu
Evolution ◽  
1986 ◽  
Vol 40 (6) ◽  
pp. 1338-1344 ◽  
Author(s):  
David N. Reznick ◽  
Elgin Perry ◽  
Joseph Travis

Oecologia ◽  
1987 ◽  
Vol 74 (3) ◽  
pp. 458-467 ◽  
Author(s):  
W. V. Reid

2015 ◽  
Vol 282 (1821) ◽  
pp. 20151808 ◽  
Author(s):  
Paola Laiolo ◽  
Javier Seoane ◽  
Juan Carlos Illera ◽  
Giulia Bastianelli ◽  
Luis María Carrascal ◽  
...  

The fit between life histories and ecological niche is a paradigm of phenotypic evolution, also widely used to explain patterns of species co-occurrence. By analysing the lifestyles of a sympatric avian assemblage, we show that species' solutions to environmental problems are not unbound. We identify a life-history continuum structured on the cost of reproduction along a temperature gradient, as well as habitat-driven parental behaviour. However, environmental fit and trait convergence are limited by niche filling and by within-species variability of niche traits, which is greater than variability of life histories. Phylogeny, allometry and trade-offs are other important constraints: lifetime reproductive investment is tightly bound to body size, and the optimal allocation to reproduction for a given size is not established by niche characteristics but by trade-offs with survival. Life histories thus keep pace with habitat and climate, but under the limitations imposed by metabolism, trade-offs among traits and species' realized niche.


2017 ◽  
Vol 30 (1) ◽  
pp. e23069 ◽  
Author(s):  
Anna Ziomkiewicz ◽  
Amara Frumkin ◽  
Yawei Zhang ◽  
Amelia Sancilio ◽  
Richard G. Bribiescas

1982 ◽  
Vol 9 (1) ◽  
pp. 145 ◽  
Author(s):  
SM Pellis ◽  
VC Pellis

The vigilance behaviour of geese was measured by the amount of time per day they spent with their heads raised, and also by the frequency with which they interrupted feeding to look up. Goslings were vulnerable to predation in the first 4 weeks after hatching. The adults' vigilance behaviour was higher during the 4 weeks after hatching than later, and this reduced the amount of time they spent feeding. It is argued that brood size and the adults' ability to protect the young are inversely related, and that larger broods, for this reason, raise the cost of reproduction. These factors are considered as posthatching limitations on clutch size.


Oecologia ◽  
2019 ◽  
Vol 191 (2) ◽  
pp. 369-375
Author(s):  
Danielle A. Sherman ◽  
Johan P. Dahlgren ◽  
Johan Ehrlén ◽  
María Begoña García

1988 ◽  
Vol 66 (8) ◽  
pp. 1906-1912 ◽  
Author(s):  
Todd W. Arnold

Recently, Zammuto (R. M. Zammuto. 1986. Can. J. Zool. 64: 2739–2749) suggested that North American game birds exhibited survival–fecundity trade-offs consistent with the "cost of reproduction" hypothesis. However, there were four serious problems with the data and the analyses that Zammuto used: (i) the species chosen for analysis ("game birds") showed little taxonomic or ecological uniformity, (ii) the measures of future reproductive value (maximum longevity) were severely biased by unequal sample sizes of band recoveries, (iii) the measures of current reproductive effort (clutch sizes) were inappropriate given that most of the birds analyzed produce self-feeding precocial offspring, and (iv) the statistical units used in the majority of analyses (species) were not statistically independent with respect to higher level taxonomy. After correcting these problems, I found little evidence of survival–fecundity trade-offs among precocial game birds, and I attribute most of the explainable variation in life-history traits of these birds to allometry, phylogeny, and geography.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document