scholarly journals Feature-binding errors during saccadic remapping may affect perception of real-world objects

2020 ◽  
Vol 20 (11) ◽  
pp. 595
Author(s):  
Christopher M Jones ◽  
Julie D Golomb
2015 ◽  
Vol 15 (13) ◽  
pp. 8 ◽  
Author(s):  
Natalie N. Stepien ◽  
Steven K. Shevell

1997 ◽  
Vol 9 (3) ◽  
pp. 295-317 ◽  
Author(s):  
Lynn Robertson ◽  
Anne Treisman ◽  
Stacia Friedman-Hill ◽  
Marcia Grabowecky

An earlier report described a patient (RM) with bilateral parietal damage who showed severe binding problems between shape and color and shape and size (Friedman-Hill, Robertson, & Treisman, 1995). When shown two different-colored letters, RM reported a large number of illusory conjunctions (ICs) combining the shape of one letter with the color of the other, even when he was looking directly at one of them and had as long as 10 sec to respond. The lesions also produced severe deficits in locating and reaching for objects, and difficulty in seeing more than one object at a time, resulting in a neuropsychological diagnosis of Balint's syndrome or dorsal simultanagnosia. The pattern of deficits supported predictions of Treisman's Feature Integration Theory (FIT) that the loss of spatial information would lead to binding errors. They further suggested that the spatial information used in binding depends on intact parietal function. In the present paper we extend these findings and examine other deficits in RM that would be predicted by FIT. We show that: (1) Object individuation is impaired, making it impossible for him correctly to count more than one or two objects, even when he is aware that more are present. (2) Visual search for a target defined by a conjunction of features (requiring binding) is impaired, while the detection of a target defined by a unique feature is not. Search for the absence of a feature (0 among Qs) is also severely impaired, while search for the presence (Q among 0s) is not. Feature absence can only be detected when all the present features are bound to the nontarget items. (3) RM's deficits cannot be attributed to a general binding problem: binding errors were far more likely with simultaneous presentation where spatial information was required than with sequential presentation where time could be used as the medium for binding. (4) Selection for attention was severely impaired, whether it was based on the position of a marker or on some other feature (color). (5) Spatial information seems to exist that RM cannot access, suggesting that feature binding relies on a relatively late stage where implicit spatial information is made explicitly accessible. The data converge to support our conclusions that explicit spatial knowledge is necessary for the perception of accurately bound features, for accurate attentional selection, and for accurate and rapid search for a conjunction of features in a multiitem display. It is obviously necessary for directing attention to spatial locations, but the consequences of impairments in this ability seem also to affect object selection, object individuation, and feature integration. Thus, the functional effects of parietal damage are not limited to the spatial and attentional problems that have long been described in patients with Balint's syndrome. Damage to parietal areas also affects object perception through damage to spatial representations that are fundamental for spatial awareness.


2014 ◽  
Vol 25 (5) ◽  
pp. 1067-1078 ◽  
Author(s):  
Julie D. Golomb ◽  
Zara E. L’Heureux ◽  
Nancy Kanwisher

2021 ◽  
Vol 21 (9) ◽  
pp. 1937
Author(s):  
Lasyapriya Pidaparthi ◽  
Jiageng Chen ◽  
Andrew B Leber ◽  
Julie D Golomb

2014 ◽  
Vol 14 (10) ◽  
pp. 1030-1030
Author(s):  
J. Golomb

2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Bahiya Kewan ◽  
Amit Yashar

Crowding refers to the failure to identify a peripheral object due to nearby objects (flankers). A hallmark of crowding is the inner-outer asymmetry, i.e., the outer flanker (more peripheral) produces stronger interference than the inner one. Here, by manipulating attention, we tested the predictions of two competing accounts: the attentional account, which predicts a positive attentional effect on the asymmetry (i.e., attention to the outer flanker will increase the asymmetry), and the receptive field size account, which predicts a negative attentional effect. In Experiment 1, observers estimated a Gabor target orientation. A peripheral pre-cue draws attention to one of three locations: target, inner or outer flanker. Probabilistic mixture modeling demonstrated the asymmetry by showing that observers often misreported the outer flanker orientation as the target. Interestingly, the outer cue led to a higher misreport rate of the outer flanker, and the inner cue led to a lower misreport rate of the outer flanker. Experiment 2 tested the effect of asymmetry and attention on binding errors (e.g., reporting the tilt of one presented item with the color of another item). Observers reported both the tilt and color of the target. Attention increased target reports in both dimensions and led to a decrease in target binding. However, attention did not lead to a decrease in flanker biding errors. The results are consistent with the attentional account of crowding and suggest that the locus of spatial attention plays an essential role in crowding and the inner-outer asymmetry.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document