scholarly journals Anticipatory smooth eye movements adapt to higher-order probabilistic structure of the environment

2021 ◽  
Vol 21 (9) ◽  
pp. 2003
Author(s):  
Vanessa Carneiro Morita ◽  
Guillaume S Masson ◽  
Anna Montagnini
2004 ◽  
Vol 92 (3) ◽  
pp. 1501-1511 ◽  
Author(s):  
G. R. Barnes ◽  
G. D. Paige

We compared the predictive behavior of smooth pursuit (SP) and suppression of the vestibuloocular reflex (VOR) in humans by examining anticipatory smooth eye movements, a phenomenon that arises after repeated presentations of sudden target movement preceded by an auditory warning cue. We investigated whether anticipatory smooth eye movements also occur prior to cued head motion, particularly when subjects expect interaction between the VOR and either real or imagined head-fixed targets. Subjects were presented with horizontal motion stimuli consisting of a visual target alone (SP), head motion in darkness (VOR), or head motion in the presence of a real or imagined head-fixed target (HFT and IHFT, respectively). Stimulus sequences were delivered as single cycles of a velocity sinusoid (frequency: 0.5 or 1.0 Hz) that were either cued (a sound cue 400 ms earlier) or noncued. For SP, anticipatory smooth eye movements developed over repeated trials in the cued, but not the noncued, condition. In the VOR condition, no such anticipatory eye movements were observed even when cued. In contrast, anticipatory responses were observed under cued, but not noncued, HFT and IHFT conditions, as for SP. Anticipatory HFT responses increased in proportion to the velocity of preceding stimuli. In general, anticipatory gaze responses were similar in cued SP, HFT, and IHFT conditions and were appropriate for expected target motion in space. Anticipatory responses may represent the output of a central mechanism for smooth-eye-movement generation that operates during predictive SP as well as VOR modulations that are linked with SP even in the absence of real visual targets.


2015 ◽  
Vol 15 (12) ◽  
pp. 1018 ◽  
Author(s):  
Eileen Kowler ◽  
Lakshmi Kolisetty ◽  
Cordelia Aitkin ◽  
Nicholas Ross ◽  
Elio Santos ◽  
...  

2015 ◽  
Vol 15 (12) ◽  
pp. 1019
Author(s):  
Jean-Bernard Damasse ◽  
Laurent Madelain ◽  
Laurent Perrinet ◽  
Anna Montagnini

2019 ◽  
Vol 5 (1) ◽  
pp. 223-246 ◽  
Author(s):  
Eileen Kowler ◽  
Jason F. Rubinstein ◽  
Elio M. Santos ◽  
Jie Wang

Smooth pursuit eye movements maintain the line of sight on smoothly moving targets. Although often studied as a response to sensory motion, pursuit anticipates changes in motion trajectories, thus reducing harmful consequences due to sensorimotor processing delays. Evidence for predictive pursuit includes ( a) anticipatory smooth eye movements (ASEM) in the direction of expected future target motion that can be evoked by perceptual cues or by memory for recent motion, ( b) pursuit during periods of target occlusion, and ( c) improved accuracy of pursuit with self-generated or biologically realistic target motions. Predictive pursuit has been linked to neural activity in the frontal cortex and in sensory motion areas. As behavioral and neural evidence for predictive pursuit grows and statistically based models augment or replace linear systems approaches, pursuit is being regarded less as a reaction to immediate sensory motion and more as a predictive response, with retinal motion serving as one of a number of contributing cues.


1986 ◽  
Vol 63 (2) ◽  
pp. 463-469 ◽  
Author(s):  
Christine L. Allegretti ◽  
J. Thomas Puglisi

12 disabled and 12 nondisabled readers (mean age, 11 yr.) were compared on a letter-search task which separated perceptual processing from higher-order processing. Participants were presented a first stimulus (for 200 msec. to minimize eye movements) followed by a second stimulus immediately to estimate the amount of information initially perceived or after a 3000-msec. interval to examine information more permanently stored. Participants were required to decide whether any letter present in the first stimulus was also present in the second. Two processing loads (1 and 3 letters) were examined. Disabled readers showed more pronounced deficits when they were given very little time to process information or more information to process.


2012 ◽  
Vol 12 (11) ◽  
pp. 1-1 ◽  
Author(s):  
E. M. Santos ◽  
E. K. Gnang ◽  
E. Kowler

2000 ◽  
Vol 13 (2-3) ◽  
pp. 201-214 ◽  
Author(s):  
Christoph Zetzsche ◽  
Kerstin Schill ◽  
Gerhard Krieger ◽  
Gert Hauske ◽  
Ingo Rentschler

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document