scholarly journals Figure-Ground Organization in Natural Scenes: Performance of a Recurrent Neural Model Compared with Neurons of Area V2

eNeuro ◽  
2019 ◽  
Vol 6 (3) ◽  
pp. ENEURO.0479-18.2019 ◽  
Author(s):  
Brian Hu ◽  
Rüdiger von der Heydt ◽  
Ernst Niebur
2016 ◽  
Author(s):  
Jonathan R. Williford ◽  
Rüdiger von der Heydt

AbstractFigure-ground organization and border-ownership assignment are essential for understanding natural scenes. It has been shown that many neurons in the macaque visual cortex signal border-ownership in displays of simple geometric shapes such as squares, but how well these neurons resolve border-ownership in natural scenes is not known. We studied area V2 neurons in behaving macaques with static images of complex natural scenes. We found that about half of the neurons were border-ownership selective for contours in natural scenes and this selectivity originated from the image context. The border-ownership signals emerged within 70 ms after stimulus onset, only ~30 ms after response onset. A substantial fraction of neurons were highly consistent across scenes. Thus, the cortical mechanisms of figure-ground organization are fast and efficient even in images of complex natural scenes. Understanding how the brain performs this task so fast remains a challenge.Significance StatementHere we show, for the first time, that neurons in primate visual area V2 signal border-ownership for objects in complex natural scenes. Surprisingly, these signals appear as early as the border-ownership signals for simple figure displays. In fact, they emerge well before object selective activity appears in infero-temporal cortex, which rules out feedback from that region as an explanation. Thus, “objectness” is detected by extremely fast mechanisms that do not depend on feedback from the known object-recognition centers.


eNeuro ◽  
2016 ◽  
Vol 3 (6) ◽  
pp. ENEURO.0127-16.2016 ◽  
Author(s):  
Jonathan R. Williford ◽  
Rüdiger von der Heydt

2007 ◽  
Vol 97 (6) ◽  
pp. 4310-4326 ◽  
Author(s):  
Edward Craft ◽  
Hartmut Schütze ◽  
Ernst Niebur ◽  
Rüdiger von der Heydt

Psychophysical studies suggest that figure–ground organization is a largely autonomous process that guides—and thus precedes—allocation of attention and object recognition. The discovery of border-ownership representation in single neurons of early visual cortex has confirmed this view. Recent theoretical studies have demonstrated that border-ownership assignment can be modeled as a process of self-organization by lateral interactions within V2 cortex. However, the mechanism proposed relies on propagation of signals through horizontal fibers, which would result in increasing delays of the border-ownership signal with increasing size of the visual stimulus, in contradiction with experimental findings. It also remains unclear how the resulting border-ownership representation would interact with attention mechanisms to guide further processing. Here we present a model of border-ownership coding based on dedicated neural circuits for contour grouping that produce border-ownership assignment and also provide handles for mechanisms of selective attention. The results are consistent with neurophysiological and psychophysical findings. The model makes predictions about the hypothetical grouping circuits and the role of feedback between cortical areas.


Perception ◽  
1997 ◽  
Vol 26 (1_suppl) ◽  
pp. 367-367
Author(s):  
L Spillmann

Gestalt psychologists in the early part of the century challenged psychophysical notions that perceptual phenomena can be understood from a punctate (‘atomistic’) analysis of the elements present in the stimulus. Their ideas also inhibited later attempts to explain vision in terms of single-unit recordings from individual neurons. A rapprochement between Gestalt phenomenology and physiology seemed unlikely when the first ECVP was held in Marburg, Germany, in 1978. Since that time, response properties of neurons have been discovered that invite an interpretation of visual phenomena (including ‘illusions’) in terms of neuronal processing. Indeed, it is now possible to understand some Gestalt phenomena on the basis of known neurophysiological mechanisms. I begin by outlining the great strides that have been made since the advent of microelectrode recording from single neurons. Initially, cells (‘detectors’) selectively responding to the contrast, spatial frequency, wavelength, orientation, movement, and disparity of a stimulus placed in their receptive fields were used to interpret simple perceptual phenomena (eg, Mach bands, Hermann grids, tilt aftereffect, MAE). In recent years, cells at higher levels of the visual system have been discovered that might explain a number of more complex phenomena: the perception of illusory (occluded) contours by end-stopped cells in area V2, the filling-in of artificial scotomata by neurons in V3, colour constancy by ‘perceptive’ neurons in V4, and the perception of coherent motion in dynamic noise patterns by cells in MT. Studies of flow fields and biological motion in area MST have recently been added to account for our perceptions as we move through our environment. Prompted by these findings, a shift from local to global interactions ‘beyond the classical receptive field’ has taken place in our search for the neural substrates of perception. Current research has focused on three kinds of mechanisms: (i) converging feed-forward projections as the basis for new response properties emerging at higher levels, (ii) recruitment of lateral connections to explain filling-in, and (iii) backward propagation from higher to lower levels to account for binding and figure - ground segregation. How such mechanisms compute large-scale surface properties such as brightness, colour, and depth from local features—indeed how they construct the surfaces themselves from complex natural scenes—is only one of the many questions that are under scrutiny today. Future research will have to tackle the all-important question: How does the analysed information come together again? Furthermore, the contributions of eye movements, attention, learning, other sense modalities, and motor actions will have to be taken into consideration before we arrive at a more complete understanding of visual perception.


2014 ◽  
Vol 103 ◽  
pp. 116-126 ◽  
Author(s):  
Sudarshan Ramenahalli ◽  
Stefan Mihalas ◽  
Ernst Niebur

2004 ◽  
Vol 4 (8) ◽  
pp. 197-197 ◽  
Author(s):  
T. Sugihara ◽  
F. T. Qiu ◽  
R. Heydt
Keyword(s):  
Area V2 ◽  

1995 ◽  
Author(s):  
S.N. Yendrikhovskij ◽  
H. DE Ridder ◽  
E.A. Fedorovskaya

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document