Effects of Non-Anticipation and Anticipation Procedures upon Paired-Associate Learning in Unmixed and Mixed List Designs

1964 ◽  
Vol 15 (3) ◽  
pp. 795-801 ◽  
Author(s):  
James H. Reynolds

Two experiments compared verbal PA learning by the standard anticipation technique with learning by a non-anticipation method in which immediate confirmation O- correct responding was eliminated. Most previous investigations have found that learning by the latter procedure is superior to learning by the usual anticipation method. In Exp. I, which employed an unmixed list design, no differences in learning were obtained between the two methods at either of two levels of list difficulty. However, Exp. II, using the same materials in a mixed list design, showed superior learning of items presented by the non-anticipation method regardless of the difficulty of the list. The conflicting results of the two experiments suggest that evidence for superior verbal PA learning by the non-anticipation method may depend, at least in part, upon the list design employed.

1967 ◽  
Vol 20 (3_suppl) ◽  
pp. 1191-1200 ◽  
Author(s):  
Chizuko Izawa

Investigations of a new experimental variable from the arrangements of reinforcements (R) and tests (T) in paired-associate learning were furthered by a 2 × 2 × 2 factorial experimental design: 64 college students learned two lists of 12 pairs, one with unmixed list (Exp. I) and the other with mixed list (Exp. II). Four repetitive experimental sequences in each experiment were RTRT …, RRTRRT. … RTTRTT …, and RRTTRRTT. … No significant differences were found between mixed- and unmixed-list designs for any given statistic examined. The findings indicate that individual pairs in a given condition were learned relatively independently of those in the other conditions within a list. The present results were close replications of the previous study by Izawa (1966a) and support the stimulus fluctuation model.


1971 ◽  
Vol 4 (2) ◽  
pp. 300-300
Author(s):  
Joseph W. Gallagher ◽  
Donald R. Reid

1965 ◽  
Vol 17 (2) ◽  
pp. 491-497 ◽  
Author(s):  
Donald J. Mueller ◽  
Robert M. W. Travers

Each of 34 Ss was presented with a list of 12 paired associates which were arranged according to high-low or low-high stimulus and response meaningfulness and also in a simultaneous or sequential time relationship. Meaningfulness level on the stimulus side of the dyad rather than on the response side was found to be more crucial for learning, and significantly more learning occurred also when the dyads were presented in the simultaneous condition. The findings were discussed in terms of both association theory and the differences between the present procedure and the conventional anticipation method.


1979 ◽  
Vol 44 (2) ◽  
pp. 648-650 ◽  
Author(s):  
May F. D'amato ◽  
Mark Diamond

14 students in each of four groups learned a single unmixed list of 19 CVC pairs for 12 anticipation trials followed by a free recall of the pairs. In three of the four lists a single rule applied to all of the pairs. The rule was that the words in each pair changed first letter (rhymed), changed middle letter, or changed last letter. A fourth list contained only pairs of unrelated words. Mean number of correct anticipations per trial showed rhyming and end-change rules to be equally beneficial, although not as effective as the middle-change rule. Free recall of the pairs showed no differences among lists. Results were interpreted as supporting the hypothesis that rules facilitate retrieval by restricting the number of responses to be considered for each stimulus.


1982 ◽  
Vol 50 (3) ◽  
pp. 1009-1010
Author(s):  
Harriet M. Braunstein ◽  
Kari S. Peacock ◽  
Joan M. Soloko ◽  
Susan L. Tippit

Paired-associate items which followed an association rule were presented in a mixed list with a smaller number of irregular items which deviated from the rule but were presented for study more frequently. On test lists new stimulus items were presented for which the correct response followed the rule. In learning the items 30 subjects were instructed to use either maintenance rehearsal or elaborative operations. The prediction that the elaboration strategy would facilitate performance was confirmed. However, the expectation that maintenance rehearsal would produce equal ease of learning on irregular items was not upheld. All subjects performed best on irregular items, probably because they were more frequently presented and contained unusual response elements which may have stood out.


1965 ◽  
Vol 16 (3_suppl) ◽  
pp. 1123-1124 ◽  
Author(s):  
James H. Reynolds

Groups learning paired-associates by either the anticipation or non-anticipation method were given a single test after 6 trials and then 5 more tests in series after 9 trials. No differences between learning methods were obtained on either initial testing or the recall series. The results confirm previous findings that the methods yield equivalent learning, suggesting that informational feedback is unnecessary for verbal associative formation.


1970 ◽  
Vol 27 (2) ◽  
pp. 327-334 ◽  
Author(s):  
C. Milton Blue

A P-A (visual materials) study was conducted with 24 intellectually average and 24 educable mentally retarded individuals of high school age. Each S received 2 equivalent tasks, one in the anticipation method of presentation and the other in the recall method. The learning speed of the average Ss was significantly superior to that of the retardates. Method of presentation did not influence the learning of the average Ss but learning in the recall presentation was superior for retarded Ss.


1975 ◽  
Vol 37 (3_suppl) ◽  
pp. 1196-1198
Author(s):  
Fred A. Minnigerode

60 4-, 5- and 6-yr.-old children learned two 3-item paired-associate lists. A mixed-list design was used. List 2 contained pairs representing A-B, C-D, A-B, A-B', and A-B, A-C transfer paradigms. Terms B and B' were related via rhyme. In second-list learning, more correct responses in the first two anticipation trials occurred on A-B' pairs than on A-C pairs and more correct responses on C-D pairs than on A-B' pairs. Age differences were nonsignificant. Conditions under which children might successfully utilize mediational rhyme strategies in paired-associate learning were suggested.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document