Science-Practice Gap

Author(s):  
Scott O. Lilienfeld ◽  
Lorie A. Ritschel ◽  
Steven Jay Lynn ◽  
Robin L. Cautin ◽  
Robert D. Latzman
2019 ◽  
Author(s):  
Stacy Freheit ◽  
Gisel G. Suarez Bonilla ◽  
Christopher S. Vye ◽  
Bruce E. Clark

2017 ◽  
Vol 93 (2) ◽  
pp. 1032-1055 ◽  
Author(s):  
Diana Bertuol-Garcia ◽  
Carla Morsello ◽  
Charbel N. El-Hani ◽  
Renata Pardini

2018 ◽  
Vol 66 ◽  
pp. S28-S35 ◽  
Author(s):  
Christopher M. Callahan ◽  
Daniel R. Bateman ◽  
Sophia Wang ◽  
Malaz A. Boustani

2011 ◽  
pp. 135-153
Author(s):  
Robert J. Cabin

2015 ◽  
Vol 8 (1) ◽  
pp. 80-85 ◽  
Author(s):  
Paul E. Levy ◽  
Stanley B. Silverman ◽  
Caitlin M. Cavanaugh

The scientist–practitioner model of training in industrial and organizational psychology provides the foundation for the education of industrial and organizational psychologists across the world. This approach is important because, as industrial and organizational psychologists, we are responsible for both the creation and discovery of knowledge and the use or application of that knowledge. In multiple articles recently published in this journal, Pulakos and her colleagues (Pulakos, Mueller Hanson, Arad, & Moye, 2015; Pulakos & O’Leary, 2011) have argued that performance management (PM), as applied and implemented in organizations, is broken. This is not a unique take on the state of PM in organizations, as others have been arguing for many years that PM is no longer working in organizations the way that we would like it to work (Banks & Murphy, 1985; Bretz, Milkovich, & Read, 1992). Further, for many years and in many Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology conference panels and debates in the literature, we have been inundated with discussions and conversations around the science–practice gap and around the gap being especially evident in PM.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document