Internet-word compared with daily-word priming reduces attentional scope

2020 ◽  
Vol 238 (4) ◽  
pp. 1025-1033
Author(s):  
Ming Peng ◽  
Libin Zhang ◽  
Yiran Wen ◽  
Qingbai Zhao
2004 ◽  
Author(s):  
Christian Waugh ◽  
Kareem Johnson ◽  
Barbara Fredrickson
Keyword(s):  

Cortex ◽  
2000 ◽  
Vol 36 (1) ◽  
pp. 59-69 ◽  
Author(s):  
F YASUNO ◽  
T NISHIKAWA ◽  
H TOKUNAGA ◽  
K YOSHIYAMA ◽  
Y NAKAGAWA ◽  
...  
Keyword(s):  

2010 ◽  
Vol 10 (7) ◽  
pp. 1340-1340
Author(s):  
P. F. Bulakowski ◽  
D. Yu ◽  
S. T. L. Chung
Keyword(s):  

PLoS ONE ◽  
2019 ◽  
Vol 14 (1) ◽  
pp. e0208318
Author(s):  
Zhiheng Zhou ◽  
Carol Whitney ◽  
Lars Strother

2019 ◽  
Vol 19 (3) ◽  
pp. 586-599 ◽  
Author(s):  
Ajay Nadig ◽  
Nicholas J. Kelley ◽  
Narun Pornpattananangkul ◽  
James E. Glazer ◽  
Robin Nusslock

2021 ◽  
pp. 173-190
Author(s):  
R. Barker Bausell

But what happens to investigators whose studies fails to replicate? The answer is complicated by the growing use of social media by scientists and the tenor of the original investigators’ responses to the replicators. Alternative case studies are presented including John Bargh’s vitriolic outburst following a failure of his classic word priming study to replicate, Amy Cuddy’s unfortunate experience with power posing, and Matthew Vees’s low-keyed response in which he declined to aggressively disparage his replicators, complemented the replicators’ interpretation of their replication, and neither defended his original study or even suggested that its findings might be wrong. In addition to such case studies, surveys on the subject suggest that there are normally no long-term deleterious career or reputational effects on investigators for a failure of a study to replicate and that a reasoned (or no) response to a failed replication is the superior professional and affective solution.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document