scholarly journals On understanding creative language: The late positive complex and novel metaphor comprehension

2018 ◽  
Vol 1678 ◽  
pp. 231-244 ◽  
Author(s):  
Karolina Rataj ◽  
Anna Przekoracka-Krawczyk ◽  
Rob H.J. van der Lubbe
2014 ◽  
Vol 26 (9) ◽  
pp. 2042-2054 ◽  
Author(s):  
Eren Gunseli ◽  
Christian N. L. Olivers ◽  
Martijn Meeter

Prominent theories of attention claim that visual search is guided through attentional templates stored in working memory. Recently, the contralateral delay activity (CDA), an electrophysiological index of working memory storage, has been found to rapidly decrease when participants repeatedly search for the same target, suggesting that, with learning, the template moves out of working memory. However, this has only been investigated with pop-out search for distinct targets, for which a strong attentional template may not be necessary. More effortful search tasks might rely more on an active attentional template in working memory, leading to a slower handoff to long-term memory and thus a slower decline of the CDA. Using ERPs, we compared the rate of learning of attentional templates in pop-out and effortful search tasks. In two experiments, the rate of decrease in the CDA was the same for both search tasks. Similar results were found for a second component indexing working memory effort, the late positive complex. However, the late positive complex was also sensitive to anticipated search difficulty, as was expressed in a greater amplitude before the harder search task. We conclude that the amount of working memory effort invested in maintaining an attentional template, but not the rate of learning, depends on search difficulty.


2006 ◽  
Vol 18 (11) ◽  
pp. 1863-1876 ◽  
Author(s):  
Deborah E. Hannula ◽  
Kara D. Federmeier ◽  
Neal J. Cohen

Various lines of evidence suggest that memory for the relations among arbitrarily paired items acquired prior to testing can influence early processing of a probe stimulus. The event-related potential experiment reported here was designed to explore how early in time memory for a previously established face-scene relationship begins to influence processing of faces, under sequential presentation conditions in which a preview of the scene can promote expectancies about the to-be-presented face. Prior to the current work, the earliest component documented to be sensitive to memory for the relations among arbitrarily paired items was the late positive complex (LPC), but here relational memory effects were evident as early as 270–350 msec after face onset. The latency of these relational memory effects suggests that they may be the precursor to similar effects observed in eye movement behavior. As expected, LPC amplitude was also affected by memory for face-scene relationships, and N400 amplitude reflected some combination of memory for items and memory for the relations among items.


2005 ◽  
Vol 116 (10) ◽  
pp. 2363-2380 ◽  
Author(s):  
Jacqueline A. Rushby ◽  
Robert J. Barry ◽  
Rebecca J. Doherty

2013 ◽  
Vol 542 ◽  
pp. 76-80 ◽  
Author(s):  
Guochao Li ◽  
Suiping Wang ◽  
Yixing Duan ◽  
Zude Zhu

1982 ◽  
Vol 54 (2) ◽  
pp. 132-146 ◽  
Author(s):  
Walton T Roth ◽  
Geoffrey H Blowers ◽  
Carol M Doyle ◽  
Bert S Kopell

2016 ◽  
Vol 28 (4) ◽  
pp. 542-557 ◽  
Author(s):  
Henning Gibbons ◽  
Robert Schnuerch ◽  
Jutta Stahl

Previous studies on the neurophysiological underpinnings of feedback processing almost exclusively used low-ambiguity feedback, which does not fully address the diversity of situations in everyday life. We therefore used a pseudo trial-and-error learning task to investigate ERPs of low- versus high-ambiguity feedback. Twenty-eight participants tried to deduce the rule governing visual feedback to their button presses in response to visual stimuli. In the blocked condition, the same two feedback words were presented across several consecutive trials, whereas in the random condition feedback was randomly drawn on each trial from sets of five positive and five negative words. The feedback-related negativity (FRN-D), a frontocentral ERP difference between negative and positive feedback, was significantly larger in the blocked condition, whereas the centroparietal late positive complex indicating controlled attention was enhanced for negative feedback irrespective of condition. Moreover, FRN-D in the blocked condition was due to increased reward positivity (Rew-P) for positive feedback, rather than increased (raw) FRN for negative feedback. Our findings strongly support recent lines of evidence that the FRN-D, one of the most widely studied signatures of reinforcement learning in the human brain, critically depends on feedback discriminability and is primarily driven by the Rew-P. A novel finding concerned larger frontocentral P2 for negative feedback in the random but not the blocked condition. Although Rew-P points to a positivity bias in feedback processing under conditions of low feedback ambiguity, P2 suggests a specific adaptation of information processing in case of highly ambiguous feedback, involving an early negativity bias. Generalizability of the P2 findings was demonstrated in a second experiment using explicit valence categorization of highly emotional positive and negative adjectives.


1991 ◽  
Vol 3 (2) ◽  
pp. 128-130
Author(s):  
Yasuhiko Saito ◽  
◽  
Takuji Yamamoto ◽  
S Kanamura ◽  

It has been reported that a brief presentation of odorous stimulus has some effects on Event-related brain potentials (ERPs). In this present study, effects of continuous presentation of odorous stimulus (scented environment) were studies with special references to ERP basis waves which associated to cognitive function. Contingent negative variation (CNV) was recorded before and after continuous presentation of 0.2g lemon oil. ERP basis waves were extracted by Multivariate analysis of single trial CNVs, and it is shown that the ERP basis waves of CNV contained a number of positive components after S1 and S2 of the conventional CNV paradigm. These positive components corresponded from the view point of their latency and topographic characteristics to the late positive complex. In the scented environment by the lemon oil, Sl +350p basis wave, which is a positive basis wave developed 350msec after S1, and S2 +350p, a positive basis wave 350msec after S2, decreased their amplitude significantly and S2 +650p, the second positive basis wave after S2, was also decreased in its amplitude. On the contrary to this, there noted increased amplitude of S1 +500p and S2 +500p in the scented environment. According to the report after the experiment, the odor of lemon oil induced tranquil and hedonic feeling among the subjects and they felt rather relaxed in the scented environment. The effects on the ERP basis waves are thought to be explained along this line of affective changes in the subjects.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document