scholarly journals Higher-Order Conditioning Is Impaired by Hippocampal Lesions

2014 ◽  
Vol 24 (18) ◽  
pp. 2202-2207 ◽  
Author(s):  
Asaf Gilboa ◽  
Melanie Sekeres ◽  
Morris Moscovitch ◽  
Gordon Winocur
1969 ◽  
Vol 24 (1) ◽  
pp. 288-290 ◽  
Author(s):  
Adam W. Miller ◽  
Wylla D. Barsness

This study investigated the degree to which higher-order conditioning of word meaning demonstrated acquisition, stimulus generalization, extinction and differential effects of reinforcement schedules. For 120 Ss, using a 2 × 3 factorial design, only extinction was not demonstrated. S awareness was unrelated to acquisition.


1957 ◽  
Vol 56 (1) ◽  
pp. 67-72 ◽  
Author(s):  
J. V. Murphy ◽  
R. E. Miller

Infancy ◽  
2003 ◽  
Vol 4 (2) ◽  
pp. 157-176 ◽  
Author(s):  
S. J. Cheslock ◽  
E. I. Varlinskaya ◽  
J. M. High ◽  
N. E. Spear

2021 ◽  
Vol 15 ◽  
Author(s):  
Youcef Bouchekioua ◽  
Yutaka Kosaki ◽  
Shigeru Watanabe ◽  
Aaron P. Blaisdell

Spatial learning and memory, the processes through which a wide range of living organisms encode, compute, and retrieve information from their environment to perform goal-directed navigation, has been systematically investigated since the early twentieth century to unravel behavioral and neural mechanisms of learning and memory. Early theories about learning to navigate space considered that animals learn through trial and error and develop responses to stimuli that guide them to a goal place. According to a trial-and error learning view, organisms can learn a sequence of motor actions that lead to a goal place, a strategy referred to as response learning, which contrasts with place learning where animals learn locations with respect to an allocentric framework. Place learning has been proposed to produce a mental representation of the environment and the cartesian relations between stimuli within it—which Tolman coined the cognitive map. We propose to revisit some of the best empirical evidence of spatial inference in animals, and then discuss recent attempts to account for spatial inferences within an associative framework as opposed to the traditional cognitive map framework. We will first show how higher-order conditioning can successfully account for inferential goal-directed navigation in a variety of situations and then how vectors derived from path integration can be integrated via higher-order conditioning, resulting in the generation of higher-order vectors that explain novel route taking. Finally, implications to cognitive map theories will be discussed.


2021 ◽  
Vol 15 ◽  
Author(s):  
Arthur Prével ◽  
Ruth M. Krebs

In a new environment, humans and animals can detect and learn that cues predict meaningful outcomes, and use this information to adapt their responses. This process is termed Pavlovian conditioning. Pavlovian conditioning is also observed for stimuli that predict outcome-associated cues; a second type of conditioning is termed higher-order Pavlovian conditioning. In this review, we will focus on higher-order conditioning studies with simultaneous and backward conditioned stimuli. We will examine how the results from these experiments pose a challenge to models of Pavlovian conditioning like the Temporal Difference (TD) models, in which learning is mainly driven by reward prediction errors. Contrasting with this view, the results suggest that humans and animals can form complex representations of the (temporal) structure of the task, and use this information to guide behavior, which seems consistent with model-based reinforcement learning. Future investigations involving these procedures could result in important new insights on the mechanisms that underlie Pavlovian conditioning.


1977 ◽  
Vol 9 (3) ◽  
pp. 221-223
Author(s):  
Philip Compton ◽  
Donna White ◽  
Donald Robbins

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document