Cognitive D2D Finite Blocklength Transmissions with the Presence of Time-Selective Interference

Author(s):  
Prabhat Kumar Sharma ◽  
Prasanna Raut ◽  
Theodoros Tsiftsis ◽  
Paritosh Peshwe
1957 ◽  
Vol 33 (2) ◽  
pp. 163-171 ◽  
Author(s):  
Perry J. Culver ◽  
William V. McDermott ◽  
Chester M. Jones

2021 ◽  
pp. 216770262199831
Author(s):  
Alex Lau-Zhu ◽  
Richard N. Henson ◽  
Emily A. Holmes

Intrusive memories of a traumatic event can be reduced by a subsequent interference procedure, seemingly sparing voluntary memory for that event. This selective-interference effect has potential therapeutic benefits (e.g., for emotional disorders) and legal importance (e.g., for witness testimony). However, the measurements of intrusive memory and voluntary memory typically differ in the role of associations between a cue and the emotional memory “hotspots.” To test this, we asked participants to watch a traumatic film followed by either an interference procedure (reminder plus Tetris) or control procedure (reminder only). Measurement of intrusions (using a laboratory task) and voluntary memory (recognition for film stills) were crossed with the presence or absence of associative cues. The reminder-plus-Tetris group exhibited fewer intrusions despite comparable recognition memory, replicating the results of prior studies. Note that this selective interference did not appear to depend on associative cues. This involuntary versus voluntary memory dissociation for emotional material further supports separate-trace memory theories and has applied advantages.


1956 ◽  
Vol 104 (4) ◽  
pp. 577-587 ◽  
Author(s):  
Arthur S. Carlson ◽  
Aaron Kellner ◽  
Alan W. Bernheimer

When solutions of streptolysin O were added to Warburg flasks containing, among other constituents, suspensions of mitochondria from the myocardium of rabbits and citrate, fumarate, or alpha-ketoglutarate as the substrate, there followed regularly a sharp reduction, and eventually complete cessation, of oxygen consumption. This phenomenon was not observed when succinate was the substrate in the flasks, the finding pointing to a selective interference with DPN as the underlying change. The agent in the solutions of streptolysin O responsible for this effect was shown to be a streptococcal product, and to be non-dialyzable and heat-labile. It differed from streptolysin O in that it did not appear to require prior activation with cysteine, and its effectiveness was not diminished by treatment with cholesterol or antistreptolysin globulins.


2003 ◽  
Vol 90 (4) ◽  
pp. 2757-2762 ◽  
Author(s):  
Tatiana Pasternak ◽  
Daniel Zaksas

When asked to compare two moving stimuli separated by a delay, observers must not only identify stimulus direction but also store it in memory. We examined the properties of this storage mechanism in two macaque monkeys by sequentially presenting two random-dot stimuli, sample and test, in opposite hemifields and introducing a random-motion mask during the delay. The mask interfered with performance only at the precise location of the test, 100–200 ms after the start of the delay, and when its size and speed matched those of the remembered sample. This selective interference suggests that the representation of the motion stimulus in memory preserves its direction, speed, and size and is most fragile shortly after the completion of the encoding phase of the task. This precise preservation of sensory attributes of the motion stimulus suggests that the neural mechanisms involved in the processing of visual motion may also be involved in its storage.


Evolution ◽  
2011 ◽  
Vol 65 (12) ◽  
pp. 3339-3359 ◽  
Author(s):  
Alice A. Winn ◽  
Elizabeth Elle ◽  
Susan Kalisz ◽  
Pierre-Olivier Cheptou ◽  
Christopher G. Eckert ◽  
...  

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document