Olfactory and visual cues in host-finding in the Burnet moth, Zygaena trifolii

Author(s):  
Peter Ockenfels ◽  
Frank Schmidt
Keyword(s):  
2017 ◽  
Vol 107 (6) ◽  
pp. 828-838 ◽  
Author(s):  
P.A. Sanches ◽  
F. Santos ◽  
M.F.G.V. Peñaflor ◽  
J.M.S. Bento

AbstractTreating plants with synthetic jasmonic acid (JA) induces a defensive response similar to herbivore attack, and is a potential strategy for integrated pest management. Despite the importance of sugarcane, its JA-induced defences have not yet been studied. We investigated the effects of JA treatment on the direct and indirect resistance of sugarcane to the key-pest and specialist herbivore Diatraea saccharalis and the generalist Spodoptera frugiperda. Indirect defences were examined by testing the attraction of Cotesia flavipes, a sugarcane-borer parasitoid, to JA-induced volatile. The results showed that JA-treated sugarcane did not affect the weight gain of the two larvae. However, in dual-choice assays, both species preferred to feed on mock rather than JA-treated plants. Leaf colorimetric analyses showed that visual cues are unlikely to be involved in larval preference, whereas results from olfactometric assays revealed that D. saccharalis preferred JA-induced over mock plant volatiles. After 48 h of treatment, JA-treated plants emitted a volatile blend attractive to C. flavipes, comprised mainly of sesquiterpenes. However, the parasitoid did not discriminate JA-treated from host-damaged plant volatiles. When the wasps were given a choice between JA-treated and JA-treated + host-damaged plants, they preferred the latter, which emitted a more complex blend, suggesting that JA treatment likely does not hamper host-finding. We concluded that JA induces the emission of volatiles that are attractive to the sugarcane borer parasitoid, as well as an antixenosis type of resistance in sugarcane against the two pests, although neither volatiles nor visual cues alone are involved in the underlying mechanism.


Author(s):  
Hannah Burger ◽  
Melanie Marquardt ◽  
Katharina Babucke ◽  
Kim C. Heuel ◽  
Manfred Ayasse ◽  
...  

An effective means of finding food is crucial for organisms. Whereas specialized animals select a small number of potentially available food sources, generalists use a broader range. Specialist (oligolectic) bees forage on a small range of flowering plants for pollen and use primarily olfactory and visual cues to locate their host flowers. So far, however, little is known about the specific cues oligoleges use to discriminate between hosts and non-hosts and how floral scent compounds of hosts and non-hosts are processed in the bees’ olfactory system. In this study, we recorded physiological responses of the antennae (electroantennographic detection coupled to gas chromatography GC-EAD) and in the brain (optical imaging, GC-imaging), and studied host-finding behaviour of oligolectic Andrena vaga bees, a specialist on Salix plants. In total, we detected 37 physiologically active compounds in host and non-host scents. 4-Oxoisophorone, a common constituent in the scent of many Salix species, evoked strong responses in the antennal lobe glomeruli of A. vaga, but not the generalist honeybee Apis mellifera. The specific glomerular responses to 4-oxoisophorone in natural Salix scents reveals a high degree of specialization in A. vaga for this typical Salix odorant component. In behavioural experiments, we found olfactory cues to be the key attractants for A. vaga to Salix hosts, which are also used to discriminate between hosts and non-hosts, and demonstrated a behavioural activity for 4-oxoisophorone. A high sensitivity to floral scents enables the specialized bees to effectively find flowers and it appears that A. vaga bees are highly tuned to 4-oxoisophorone at a very low concentration.


2007 ◽  
Vol 42 (4) ◽  
pp. 558-572 ◽  
Author(s):  
Russell F. Mizell ◽  
Frank G. Martin ◽  
W. Louis Tedders

A modified Tilles trap and the pyramidal Tedders trap alone or in combination with 2-way pitfall traps were used in north Florida to determine trap capture efficiency and the roles of visual and olfactory cues in the behavior of 2 species of root-feeding weevils, Hylobius pales (Herbst) and Pachylobius picivorus (Germar) (Coleoptera: Curculionidae), and a cerambycid, Xylotrechus sagittatus (Germar) (Coleoptera: Cerambycidae). The 3 species responded strongly to olfactory cues provided by a 5:1 ratio of ethanol and turpentine. Visual cues followed olfactory cues in the host finding and selection hierarchy and were important in the trap response of H. pales and X. sagittatus, but less important for P. picivorus. The Tedders trap captured 18% of the total of the P. picivorus, >70% of the H. pales and >80% of the X. sagittatus. Response to odor and visual cues was more directed toward the point source in H. pales when compared with P. picivorus. More than 80% of P. picivorus landed some distance away from the trap and moved toward it. In contrast, less than 30% of H. pales landed away from or failed to enter the Tedders trap. The numbers of X. sagittatus trapped were greater in traps that were elevated off the ground. Response to the modified Tilles trap confirmed and supported the findings in the other trap configurations. Use of this methodology has potential for monitoring populations of these beetles and determining the behavior and biology of other insect species.


2014 ◽  
Vol 23 (3) ◽  
pp. 132-139 ◽  
Author(s):  
Lauren Zubow ◽  
Richard Hurtig

Children with Rett Syndrome (RS) are reported to use multiple modalities to communicate although their intentionality is often questioned (Bartolotta, Zipp, Simpkins, & Glazewski, 2011; Hetzroni & Rubin, 2006; Sigafoos et al., 2000; Sigafoos, Woodyatt, Tuckeer, Roberts-Pennell, & Pittendreigh, 2000). This paper will present results of a study analyzing the unconventional vocalizations of a child with RS. The primary research question addresses the ability of familiar and unfamiliar listeners to interpret unconventional vocalizations as “yes” or “no” responses. This paper will also address the acoustic analysis and perceptual judgments of these vocalizations. Pre-recorded isolated vocalizations of “yes” and “no” were presented to 5 listeners (mother, father, 1 unfamiliar, and 2 familiar clinicians) and the listeners were asked to rate the vocalizations as either “yes” or “no.” The ratings were compared to the original identification made by the child's mother during the face-to-face interaction from which the samples were drawn. Findings of this study suggest, in this case, the child's vocalizations were intentional and could be interpreted by familiar and unfamiliar listeners as either “yes” or “no” without contextual or visual cues. The results suggest that communication partners should be trained to attend to eye-gaze and vocalizations to ensure the child's intended choice is accurately understood.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document