scholarly journals The effect of spatial frequency content on parameters of eye movements

2008 ◽  
Vol 72 (6) ◽  
pp. 601-608 ◽  
Author(s):  
Marina T. Groner ◽  
Rudolf Groner ◽  
Adrian von Mühlenen
2016 ◽  
Vol 42 (8) ◽  
pp. 1066-1071 ◽  
Author(s):  
Timothy R. Jordan ◽  
Jasmine Dixon ◽  
Victoria A. McGowan ◽  
Stoyan Kurtev ◽  
Kevin B. Paterson

Emotion ◽  
2019 ◽  
Author(s):  
Markus Hausmann ◽  
Bobby R. Innes ◽  
Yan K. Birch ◽  
Robert W. Kentridge

2012 ◽  
Vol 25 (0) ◽  
pp. 121
Author(s):  
Marcia Grabowecky ◽  
Aleksandra Sherman ◽  
Satoru Suzuki

We have previously demonstrated a linear perceptual relationship between auditory amplitude-modulation (AM) rate and visual spatial-frequency using gabors as the visual stimuli. Can this frequency-based auditory–visual association influence perception of natural scenes? Participants consistently matched specific auditory AM rates to diverse visual scenes (nature, urban, and indoor). A correlation analysis indicated that higher subjective density ratings were associated with faster AM-rate matches. Furthermore, both the density ratings and AM-rate matches were relatively scale invariant, suggesting that the underlying crossmodal association is between visual coding of object-based density and auditory coding of AM rate. Based on these results, we hypothesized that concurrently presented fast (7 Hz) or slow (2 Hz) AM-rates might influence how visual attention is allocated to dense or sparse regions within a scene. We tested this hypothesis by monitoring eye movements while participants examined scenes for a subsequent memory task. To determine whether fast or slow sounds guided eye movements to specific spatial frequencies, we computed the maximum contrast energy at each fixation across 12 spatial frequency bands ranging from 0.06–10.16 cycles/degree. We found that the fast sound significantly guided eye movements toward regions of high spatial frequency, whereas the slow sound guided eye movements away from regions of high spatial frequency. This suggests that faster sounds may promote a local scene scanning strategy, acting as a ‘filter’ to individuate objects within dense regions. Our results suggest that auditory AM rate and visual object density are crossmodally associated, and that this association can modulate visual inspection of scenes.


Perception ◽  
1997 ◽  
Vol 26 (1_suppl) ◽  
pp. 162-162 ◽  
Author(s):  
R Groner ◽  
A von Mühlenen ◽  
M Groner

An experiment was conducted to examine the influence of luminance, contrast, and spatial frequency content on saccadic eye movements. 112 pictures of natural textures from Brodatz were low-pass filtered (0.04 – 0.76 cycles deg−1) and high-pass filtered (1.91 – 19.56 cycles deg−1) and varied in luminance (low and high) and contrast (low and high), resulting in eight images per texture. Circular clippings of the central parts of the images (approximately 15% of the whole image) were used as stimuli. In the condition of bottom - up processing, the eight stimuli derived from one texture were presented for 1500 ms in a circular arrangement around the fixation cross. They were followed by a briefly presented target stimulus in the centre, which in half the trials was identical to one of the eight test stimuli. Participants had to decide whether the target stimulus was identical to any of the preceding stimuli. During a trial, their eye movements were recorded by means of a Dual-Purkinje-Image eye tracker. In the top - down condition, the target stimulus was presented in each trial prior to the display of the test stimulus. It was assumed that the priming with a target produced a top - down processing of the test stimuli. The latency and landing site of the first saccade were computed and compared between the top - down and bottom - up conditions. It is hypothesised that stimulus characteristics (luminance, contrast, and spatial frequency) play a more prominent role in bottom - up processing, while top - down processing is adjusted to the particular characteristics of the prime.


2009 ◽  
Vol 49 (2) ◽  
pp. 173-181 ◽  
Author(s):  
A. Tavassoli ◽  
I. van der Linde ◽  
A.C. Bovik ◽  
L.K. Cormack

Geophysics ◽  
1993 ◽  
Vol 58 (9) ◽  
pp. 1281-1296 ◽  
Author(s):  
V. J. S. Grauch

The magnetic data set compiled for the Decade of North American Geology (DNAG) project presents an important digital data base that can be used to examine the North American crust. The data represent a patchwork from many individual airborne and marine magnetic surveys. However, the portion of data for the conterminous U.S. has problems that limit the resolution and use of the data. Now that the data are available in digital form, it is important to describe the data limitations more specifically than before. The primary problem is caused by datum shifts between individual survey boundaries. In the western U.S., the DNAG data are generally shifted less than 100 nT. In the eastern U.S., the DNAG data may be shifted by as much as 300 nT and contain regionally shifted areas with wavelengths on the order of 800 to 1400 km. The worst case is the artificial low centered over Kentucky and Tennessee produced by a series of datum shifts. A second significant problem is lack of anomaly resolution that arises primarily from using survey data that is too widely spaced compared to the flight heights above magnetic sources. Unfortunately, these are the only data available for much of the U.S. Another problem is produced by the lack of common observation surface between individual pieces of the U.S. DNAG data. The height disparities introduce variations in spatial frequency content that are unrelated to the magnetization of rocks. The spectral effects of datum shifts and the variation of spatial frequency content due to height disparities were estimated for the DNAG data for the conterminous U.S. As a general guideline for digital filtering, the most reliable features in the U.S. DNAG data have wavelengths roughly between 170 and 500 km, or anomaly half‐widths between 85 and 250 km. High‐quality, large‐region magnetic data sets have become increasingly important to meet exploration and scientific objectives. The acquisition of a new national magnetic data set with higher quality at a greater range of wavelengths is clearly in order. The best approach is to refly much of the U.S. with common specifications and reduction procedures. At the very least, magnetic data sets should be remerged digitally using available or newly flown long‐distance flight‐line data to adjust survey levels. In any case, national coordination is required to produce a consistent, high‐quality national magnetic map.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document