scholarly journals Comparing the minimum spatial-frequency content for recognizing Chinese and alphabet characters

2018 ◽  
Vol 18 (1) ◽  
pp. 1 ◽  
Author(s):  
Hui Wang ◽  
Gordon E. Legge
Emotion ◽  
2019 ◽  
Author(s):  
Markus Hausmann ◽  
Bobby R. Innes ◽  
Yan K. Birch ◽  
Robert W. Kentridge

2008 ◽  
Vol 72 (6) ◽  
pp. 601-608 ◽  
Author(s):  
Marina T. Groner ◽  
Rudolf Groner ◽  
Adrian von Mühlenen

Geophysics ◽  
1993 ◽  
Vol 58 (9) ◽  
pp. 1281-1296 ◽  
Author(s):  
V. J. S. Grauch

The magnetic data set compiled for the Decade of North American Geology (DNAG) project presents an important digital data base that can be used to examine the North American crust. The data represent a patchwork from many individual airborne and marine magnetic surveys. However, the portion of data for the conterminous U.S. has problems that limit the resolution and use of the data. Now that the data are available in digital form, it is important to describe the data limitations more specifically than before. The primary problem is caused by datum shifts between individual survey boundaries. In the western U.S., the DNAG data are generally shifted less than 100 nT. In the eastern U.S., the DNAG data may be shifted by as much as 300 nT and contain regionally shifted areas with wavelengths on the order of 800 to 1400 km. The worst case is the artificial low centered over Kentucky and Tennessee produced by a series of datum shifts. A second significant problem is lack of anomaly resolution that arises primarily from using survey data that is too widely spaced compared to the flight heights above magnetic sources. Unfortunately, these are the only data available for much of the U.S. Another problem is produced by the lack of common observation surface between individual pieces of the U.S. DNAG data. The height disparities introduce variations in spatial frequency content that are unrelated to the magnetization of rocks. The spectral effects of datum shifts and the variation of spatial frequency content due to height disparities were estimated for the DNAG data for the conterminous U.S. As a general guideline for digital filtering, the most reliable features in the U.S. DNAG data have wavelengths roughly between 170 and 500 km, or anomaly half‐widths between 85 and 250 km. High‐quality, large‐region magnetic data sets have become increasingly important to meet exploration and scientific objectives. The acquisition of a new national magnetic data set with higher quality at a greater range of wavelengths is clearly in order. The best approach is to refly much of the U.S. with common specifications and reduction procedures. At the very least, magnetic data sets should be remerged digitally using available or newly flown long‐distance flight‐line data to adjust survey levels. In any case, national coordination is required to produce a consistent, high‐quality national magnetic map.


2013 ◽  
Vol 13 (9) ◽  
pp. 1067-1067
Author(s):  
L. Muckli ◽  
L. S. Petro ◽  
H.-C. Rath ◽  
F. W. Smith

2019 ◽  
Vol 12 (4) ◽  
Author(s):  
Miroslav Dostalek ◽  
Jan Hejda ◽  
Karel Fliegel ◽  
Michaela Duchackova ◽  
Ladislav Dusek ◽  
...  

The stability of fusion was evaluated by its breakage when interocular blur differences were presented under vergence demand to healthy subjects. We presumed that these blur differences cause suppression of the more blurred image (interocular blur suppression, IOBS), disrupt binocular fusion and suppressed eye leaves its forced vergent position. During dichoptic presentation of static grayscale images of natural scenes, the luminance contrast (mode B) or higher-spatial frequency content (mode C) or luminance contrast plus higher-spatial frequency content (mode A) were stepwise reduced in the image presented to the non-dominant eye. We studied the effect of these types of blur on fusion stability at various levels of the vergence demand. During the divergence demand, the fusion was disrupted  with approximately half blur than during convergence. Various modes  of blur influenced fusion differently. The mode C (isolated reduction of higher-spatial frequency content) violated fusion under the lowest vergence demand significantly more than either isolated or combined reduction of luminance contrast (mode B and A). According to our results, the image´s details (i.e. higher-spatial frequency content) protects binocular fusion from disruption by the lowest vergence demand.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Jeffrey D. Nador ◽  
Matteo Zoia ◽  
Matthew Pachai ◽  
Meike Ramon

Facial identity matching ability varies widely, ranging from severely deficient prosopagnosics (who exhibit profound impairments in face cognition) to so-called Super-Recognizers (SRs), possessing exceptional capacities for processing facial identity. Yet, despite the often consequential nature of face matching decisions—such as identity verification in security critical settings—ability assessments rely on simple performance metrics on a handful of heterogeneously related subprocesses, or in some cases only a single measured subprocess. Unfortunately, methodologies of this ilk leave contributions of stimulus information to observed variations in ability largely unspecified. Moreover, they are inadequate for addressing the qualitative or quantitative nature of differences between SRs’ abilities and those of the general population. Here, therefore, we sought to investigate individual differences—among SRs identified using a novel conservative diagnostic framework, and neurotypical controls—by systematically varying retinal availability, bandwidth, and orientation of faces’ spatial frequency content over two face matching experiments. Psychophysical evaluations of these parameters’ contributions to ability reveal that SRs more consistently exploit the same spatial frequency information, rather than suggesting qualitatively different profiles between normal observers and SRs. These findings stress the importance of optimizing procedures for SR diagnosis to include measures of individuals’ consistency.


2010 ◽  
Vol 103 (3) ◽  
pp. 1606-1613 ◽  
Author(s):  
Rosanne M. van Diepen ◽  
Sabine Born ◽  
David Souto ◽  
Angélique Gauch ◽  
Dirk Kerzel

External transients, such as a flash or a startling sound, are believed to capture attention. Bauer, Cheadle, Parton, Müller, and Usher reported that attention can also be captured by a stimulus that flickers subliminally at 50 Hz, presumably by entrainment of neurons to the flicker frequency. In their reaction time (RT) task, participants had to locate a subtle change in the spatial frequency content of one of three Gabors (the target). Prior to target onset, presumably subliminal 50-Hz flicker in one of the Gabors served as a spatial cue. Bauer et al. found faster RTs when the cued location was congruent with the target location than when the cue was incongruent with the target location. In their experiments, the cue stopped to flicker at 50 Hz at target onset and was replaced by a stimulus flickering at 100 Hz (i.e., the screen refresh rate). In the present study, we show that the transition from 50 to 100 Hz results in a flash-like impression that can be localized above chance. We suggest that the illusory transition flash interfered with the localization of the subtle target, which contributed to the congruency effect. In support of this view, participants selected the flickering object more often than the non-flickering object when they failed to respond to the target. Further, no cueing effects were observed when the cue continued to flicker until the end of the trial or when the target was a salient change in polarity. In our view, the cueing effect occurs because observers confuse the illusory transition flash with the target when the two are similar. When truly subliminal flicker is used (70-Hz flicker), very small cueing effects persist in the absence of an illusory transition flash but may be accounted for by small effects on reaction time unrelated to attention.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document