scholarly journals Blue jays, Cyanocitta cristata, devalue social information in uncertain environments

2016 ◽  
Vol 112 ◽  
pp. 53-62 ◽  
Author(s):  
V.K. Heinen ◽  
D.W. Stephens
Science ◽  
1977 ◽  
Vol 195 (4278) ◽  
pp. 580-582 ◽  
Author(s):  
A. T. PIETREWICZ ◽  
A. C. KAMIL

2005 ◽  
Vol 41 (2) ◽  
pp. 354-362 ◽  
Author(s):  
Samantha E. J. Gibbs ◽  
Angela E. Ellis ◽  
Daniel G. Mead ◽  
Andrew B. Allison ◽  
J. Kevin Moulton ◽  
...  

2018 ◽  
Vol 11 (3) ◽  
pp. 159-166 ◽  
Author(s):  
Julie A. Dahl ◽  
Gary Ritchison

Some species of birds use their vocalisations to communicate predator presence and the level of threat they pose, including two species of corvids (Corvidae), American Crows (Corvus brachyrhynchos) and Siberian Jays (Perisoreus infaustus). Our objective was to determine if Blue Jays (Cyanocitta cristata), another corvid, also use specific calls or vary the characteristics of certain calls to convey information about the level of threat posed by aerial predators. During the non-breeding seasons of 2014 and 2015, we recorded and analysed the vocal responses of Blue Jays to study skins of six species of raptors that varied in size and the level of threat they pose to Blue Jays. Experiments were conducted at seven locations in Madison County, Kentucky. The mean number of Blue Jays present during trials was 2.6, and Blue Jays uttered five different vocalisations during trials, with ditonal and monotonal jeers given most frequently. The rate at which Blue Jays uttered ditonal jeers differed among trials, with rates highest during trials with an Eastern Screech-Owl (Megascops asio) and a Sharp-shinned Hawk (Accipiter striatus). However, the characteristics of ditonal and monotonal jeers (duration, low frequency, high frequency, and peak frequency) did not differ among trials. These results suggest that Blue Jays may either perceive Eastern Screech-Owls and Sharp-shinned Hawks as the greatest threats or, alternatively, as potential, but less threatening predators, and, therefore, they were willing to take greater risks when mobbing them. In contrast, Blue Jays mobbed the other raptors, i.e. American Kestrels (Falco sparverius), Red-tailed Hawks (Buteo jamaicensis), and Great Horned Owls (Bubo virginianus), with much less intensity, likely because they posed less of a threat or, in the case of Cooper's Hawks (Accipiter cooperii) that are known predators of Blue Jays, perhaps because mobbing with greater intensity, e.g. approaching more closely, posed too great a risk. Blue Jays in our study used the same calls with the same characteristics when responding to potential predators, only calling rates differed. However, such variation in calling rates when mobbing would likely provide useful information about the presence of, and possibly the threat posed by, potential predators for conspecifics and, perhaps, heterospecifics.


2020 ◽  
Vol 117 (19) ◽  
pp. 10388-10396 ◽  
Author(s):  
Richard P. Mann

Collective decisions can emerge from individual-level interactions between members of a group. These interactions are often seen as social feedback rules, whereby individuals copy the decisions they observe others making, creating a coherent group decision. The benefit of these behavioral rules to the individual agent can be understood as a transfer of information, whereby a focal individual learns about the world by gaining access to the information possessed by others. Previous studies have analyzed this exchange of information by assuming that all agents share common goals. While differences in information and differences in preferences have often been conflated, little is known about how differences between agents’ underlying preferences affect the use and efficacy of social information. In this paper, I develop a model of social information use by rational agents with differing preferences, and demonstrate that the resulting collective behavior is strongly dependent on the structure of preference sharing within the group, as well as the quality of information in the environment. In particular, I show that strong social responses are expected by individuals that are habituated to noisy, uncertain environments where private information about the world is relatively weak. Furthermore, by investigating heterogeneous group structures, I demonstrate a potential influence of cryptic minority subgroups that may illuminate the empirical link between personality and leadership.


The Auk ◽  
2002 ◽  
Vol 119 (4) ◽  
pp. 971-983 ◽  
Author(s):  
Keith A. Tarvin ◽  
Mary C. Garvin

Bird-Banding ◽  
1967 ◽  
Vol 38 (1) ◽  
pp. 77
Author(s):  
Lloyd A. Mitterling

2020 ◽  
Vol 134 (1) ◽  
pp. 71-83 ◽  
Author(s):  
Olga F. Lazareva ◽  
Kristy Gould ◽  
Jamie Linert ◽  
Damien Caillaud ◽  
Regina Paxton Gazes

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document