Compressed file length predicts search time and errors on visual displays

2005 ◽  
Author(s):  
D. C. Donderi ◽  
Sharon McFadden
Keyword(s):  
Author(s):  
Craig Haimson ◽  
John R. Anderson

We reduced time to detect target symbols in mock radar screens by partitioning displays in accordance with task instructions. Targets appeared among distractor symbols either close to or far from the display center, and participants were instructed to find the target closest to the center. Search time increased with both number of distractors and distance of target from center, and the effect of distractors was considerably greater for far than close targets. However, when close and far regions were delineated by a centrally-presented “range ring”, the distractor effect was substantially reduced, especially for far targets. We suggest that range rings focus attention on specific regions of the screen and aid in the determining of which regions have already been searched.


Displays ◽  
2005 ◽  
Vol 26 (2) ◽  
pp. 71-78 ◽  
Author(s):  
D.C. Donderi ◽  
Sharon McFadden
Keyword(s):  

Author(s):  
Donald L. Fisher ◽  
Bruce G. Coury ◽  
Tammy O. Tengs ◽  
Susan A. Duffy

Office computer users frequently need to search long lists of options for a particular target. Highlighting a subset of the options can shorten the average display search time. The goal of the current research was to identify the subset of options to highlight that minimizes users' average search time for any given display. To begin, a mathematical model of the visual search process was constructed which could be used to predict the average search time for a target in a display with or without highlighted options. The predictions of this model were then compared with the results of a visual search experiment that varied the type of highlighting (blocked, random, or no highlighting), the number of highlighted options, and the probability that a target was highlighted. Overall the predictions of the model were consistent with the results from the experiment. Finally, procedures based on the model were developed which could be used to find the optimal subset of options to highlight for a given display— that is, the subset of highlighted options that minimizes users' search times.


2000 ◽  
Author(s):  
Kevin Jordan ◽  
Robert S. McCann ◽  
Tuan Q. Tran
Keyword(s):  

1971 ◽  
Vol 36 (3) ◽  
pp. 397-409 ◽  
Author(s):  
Rachel E. Stark

Real-time amplitude contour and spectral displays were used in teaching speech production skills to a profoundly deaf, nonspeaking boy. This child had a visual attention problem, a behavior problem, and a poor academic record. In individual instruction, he was first taught to produce features of speech, for example, friction, nasal, and stop, which are present in vocalizations of 6- to 9-month-old infants, and then to combine these features in syllables and words. He made progress in speech, although sign language and finger spelling were taught at the same time. Speech production skills were retained after instruction was terminated. The results suggest that deaf children are able to extract information about the features of speech from visual displays, and that a developmental sequence should be followed as far as possible in teaching speech production skills to them.


1961 ◽  
Author(s):  
Milton H. Hodge ◽  
Morris J. Crawford ◽  
Mary L. Piercy

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document