scholarly journals Effects of directional and neutral category labels in bidimensional rule-learning problems

1974 ◽  
Vol 2 (4) ◽  
pp. 695-699 ◽  
Author(s):  
Paul G. Neumann
1994 ◽  
Vol 37 (2) ◽  
pp. 389-398 ◽  
Author(s):  
Phil J. Connell ◽  
C. Addison Stone

Two groups of children were exposed to instances of a nonlinguistic conceptual rule under controlled instructional conditions to determine whether the problems children with specific language impairment (SLI) have learning and accessing language rules extend beyond the language domain into the general cognitive domain. The performance of 20 children with SLI, aged 5:0 to 6:11 (years:months), was compared to that of 20 normally developing children matched for age and nonverbal ability. These children were taught under two instructional conditions that differed only in whether the child was asked to imitate the solution to a conceptual problem after each demonstration (imitation) or merely to observe it (modeling). Contrary to previous findings regarding linguistic rule-learning using auditory or visual symbol systems and similar instructional conditions, no difference was found between the extent of overall learning displayed by the normally developing children and those with SLI. Also, the performance of the children with SLI was not uniquely better under the imitation condition than under modeling, as had been the case with the learning of a novel morpheme in an auditory linguistic task. These results are interpreted as confirming the earlier assumption that the generally lower overall learning rate of the children with SLI on both the auditory and visual tasks reflects a specific linguistic rule-learning difficulty, rather than a general deficiency in rule induction.


1974 ◽  
Vol 35 (2) ◽  
pp. 955-962
Author(s):  
Tom Ciborowski

Three different groups of college age Ss received biconditional rule-learning problems that were altered in such a way as to permit a direct test of an unpublished model of Ss' behavior proposed by C. K. Sawyer and P. Johnson and substantially extended by Salatas and Bourne (1972). The present experiment obtained strong support for the model and evidence to support the widely reported suggestion that the principal difficulty with a biconditional rule is that S must learn to classify together two groups of elements that share no elements in common. The major outcome of the experiment was that strong empirical support was obtained for a useful but arbitrary assumption by Salatas and Bourne concerning a metric for evaluating biconditional rule-difficulty.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Ansgar D Endress

As simpler scientific theories are preferable to more convoluted ones, it is plausible to assume (and widely assumed, especially in recent Bayesian models of cognition) that biological learners are also guided by simplicity considerations when acquiring mental representations, and that formal measures of complexity might indicate which learning problems are harder and which ones are easier. However, the history of science suggests that simpler scientific theories are not necessarily more useful if more convoluted ones make calculations easier. Here, I suggest that a similar conclusion applies to mental representations. Using case studies from perception, associative learning and rule learning, I show that formal measures of complexity critically depend on assumptions about the underlying representational and processing primitives and are generally unrelated to what is actually easy to learn and process in humans. An empirically viable notion of complexity thus need to take into consideration the representational and processing primitives that are available to actual learners even if this leads to formally complex explanations.


1986 ◽  
Vol 2 (2) ◽  
pp. 199-205 ◽  
Author(s):  
Douglas Degelman ◽  
John U. Free ◽  
Michelle Scarlato ◽  
Janice M. Blackburn ◽  
Thomas Golden

The ability of kindergarten children to solve rule-learning problems following five weeks of LOGO computer experience was compared with that of children not having such experience. Fifteen children were randomly assigned either to a LOGO experience condition or a wait-list control condition. A single-keystroke LOGO was gradually introduced to the children in the LOGO condition. All children were subsequently tested on affirmative and conjunctive rule-learning tasks. Children receiving LOGO instruction had a significantly higher proportion of correct responses on two problem-solving tasks than children in the control condition ( p < .05). Frequencies of subjects correctly solving each problem revealed no statistically significant differences between conditions ( p's > .10). The kindergarten children adapted easily to the use of the computer and the single-keystroke LOGO. Suggestions for future research are presented.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document