competition group
Recently Published Documents


TOTAL DOCUMENTS

5
(FIVE YEARS 1)

H-INDEX

1
(FIVE YEARS 0)

2020 ◽  
Vol 12 (18) ◽  
pp. 7498
Author(s):  
Boheng Wang ◽  
Yuankun Bu ◽  
Guanhu Tao ◽  
Chenran Yan ◽  
Xiaolu Zhou ◽  
...  

Competition is an essential driving factor that influences forest community sustainability, yet measuring it poses several challenges. To date, the Competition Index (CI) has generally been the tool of choice for quantifying actual competition. In this study, we proposed using the Total Overlap Index (TOI), a CI in which the Area Overlap (AO) index has been adapted and modified to consider the “shading” and “crowding” effects in the vertical dimension. Next, based on six mixed forest plots in Xiaolong Mountain, Gansu, China, we assessed the results to determine the TOI’s evaluation capability. Individual-tree simulation results showed that compared to the modified Area Overlap index (AOM), the TOI has superior quantification capability in the vertical direction. The results of the basal area increment (BAI) model showed that the TOI offers the best evaluation capability among the four considered CIs in mixed forest (with Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) of 1041.60 and log-likelihood (LL) of −511.80 in the model fitting test, mean relative error of −28.67%, mean absolute percent error of 117.11%, and root mean square error of 0.7993 in cross-validation). Finally, the TOI was applied in the Kaplan–Meier survival analysis and Cox proportional-hazards analysis. The Kaplan–Meier survival analysis showed a significant difference between the low- (consisting of trees with the TOI lower than 1) and high-competition (consisting of trees with the TOI higher than 1) groups’ survival and hazard curves. Moreover, the results of the Cox proportional-hazards analysis exhibited that the trees in the low-competition group only suffered 34.29% of the hazard risk that trees in the high-competition group suffered. Overall, the TOI expresses more dimensional information than other CIs and appears to be an effective competition index for evaluating individual tree competition. Thus, the competition status quantified using this method may provide new information to guide policy- and decision-makers in sustainable forest management planning projects.


1998 ◽  
Vol 82 (1) ◽  
pp. 254-254 ◽  
Author(s):  
David C. Lundgren

In an experimental study of intergroup competition, group members ( N = 68) became more negative toward leaders who acted cooperatively toward their out-group peers and more positive toward leaders who acted antagonistically.


1978 ◽  
Vol 47 (1) ◽  
pp. 63-67 ◽  
Author(s):  
Christopher Layne

Hypotheses investigated were that signals of correctness possess incentive properties and that selective learning underestimates incentive preferences. Forty middle-class fifth graders were divided into two groups. A competition group was presented with a selective learning task, with signals of correctness and pennies as competing incentives. As predicted, they preferred the signal of correctness over the penny. A signal of correctness group was given the same task but only signals of correctness as reward. As predicted, subjects exhibited better acquisition, yielded less impulsive choice times, and chose more signals of correctness than did the competitive group. Girls increased their choices of signals of correctness and yielded faster initial choice times than did boys, suggesting sex differences in preferences.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document