Child‐father attachment in early childhood and behavior problems: A meta‐analysis

Author(s):  
Audrey‐Ann Deneault ◽  
Marian J. Bakermans‐Kranenburg ◽  
Ashley M. Groh ◽  
Pasco R. M. Fearon ◽  
Sheri Madigan
2007 ◽  
Vol 28 (1) ◽  
pp. 39-53 ◽  
Author(s):  
Ursula Pauli-Pott ◽  
Antje Haverkock ◽  
Wilfried Pott ◽  
Dieter Beckmann

Author(s):  
Christopher J. Trentacosta ◽  
Luke W. Hyde ◽  
Daniel S. Shaw ◽  
Thomas J. Dishion ◽  
Frances Gardner ◽  
...  

Infancy ◽  
2010 ◽  
Vol 15 (3) ◽  
pp. 246-269 ◽  
Author(s):  
Jie He ◽  
Kathryn Amey Degnan ◽  
Jennifer Martin McDermott ◽  
Heather A. Henderson ◽  
Amie Ashley Hane ◽  
...  

2015 ◽  
Vol 53 (3) ◽  
pp. 243-263 ◽  
Author(s):  
Holly S. Schindler ◽  
Jenya Kholoptseva ◽  
Soojin S. Oh ◽  
Hirokazu Yoshikawa ◽  
Greg J. Duncan ◽  
...  

1978 ◽  
Vol 4 (1) ◽  
pp. 31-35
Author(s):  
James A. Mcloughlin ◽  
Susan Kershman

Many teachers of young children feel unskilled or unprepared to deal with children with learning and behavior disorders. The authors suggest that at least a part of the problem lies in the teacher's orientation to her class as a group. An alternative approach is based on viewing each child in the class as an individual, on an intra-individual basis. A situational analysis approach is suggested for teachers involved in early childhood education. The use of this approach is recommended in order to accommodate the needs of each child in the group, including the child with learning and behavior problems.


2013 ◽  
Vol 25 (2) ◽  
pp. 487-500 ◽  
Author(s):  
Silvia Alemany ◽  
Frühling V. Rijsdijk ◽  
Claire Margaret Alison Haworth ◽  
Lourdes Fañanás ◽  
Robert Plomin

AbstractLittle is known about how genetic and environmental factors contribute to the association between parental negativity and behavior problems from early childhood to adolescence. The current study fitted a cross-lagged model in a sample consisting of 4,075 twin pairs to explore (a) the role of genetic and environmental factors in the relationship between parental negativity and behavior problems from age 4 to age 12, (b) whether parent-driven and child-driven processes independently explain the association, and (c) whether there are sex differences in this relationship. Both phenotypes showed substantial genetic influence at both ages. The concurrent overlap between them was mainly accounted for by genetic factors. Causal pathways representing stability of the phenotypes and parent-driven and child-driven effects significantly and independently account for the association. Significant but slight differences were found between males and females for parent-driven effects. These results were highly similar when general cognitive ability was added as a covariate. In summary, the longitudinal association between parental negativity and behavior problems seems to be bidirectional and mainly accounted for by genetic factors. Furthermore, child-driven effects were mainly genetically mediated, and parent-driven effects were a function of both genetic and shared-environmental factors.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document