Changes in the perceived duration of a narrowband sound induced by a preceding stimulus.

2009 ◽  
Vol 35 (6) ◽  
pp. 1898-1912 ◽  
Author(s):  
Robert P. Carlyon ◽  
John M. Deeks ◽  
Yury Shtyrov ◽  
Jessica Grahn ◽  
Hedwig E. Gockel ◽  
...  
2018 ◽  
Vol 44 (1) ◽  
pp. 117-127 ◽  
Author(s):  
Teresa Birngruber ◽  
Hannes Schröter ◽  
Emanuel Schütt ◽  
Rolf Ulrich
Keyword(s):  

2000 ◽  
Vol 41 (4) ◽  
pp. 481-492
Author(s):  
Naohiko Takahashi ◽  
Morio Ito ◽  
Shuji Ishida ◽  
Takao Fujino ◽  
Mikiko Nakagawa ◽  
...  

Author(s):  
Linda von Sobbe ◽  
Claudia Maienborn ◽  
Fabiola Reiber ◽  
Edith Scheifele ◽  
Rolf Ulrich
Keyword(s):  

Perception ◽  
10.1068/p6165 ◽  
2009 ◽  
Vol 38 (5) ◽  
pp. 748-762 ◽  
Author(s):  
Marie-Ève Roussel ◽  
Simon Grondin ◽  
Peter Killeen

We examined the influence of spatial factors in temporal processing. Participants categorised as short or long empty intervals marked by two brief flashes delivered from locations differing in height and depth (experiment 1), or from two of three locations on a vertical plane (experiment 2). The perceived duration of intervals, as determined by the point of subjective equality, was affected by the height and depth of the signals (experiment 1). Experiment 2 showed that the point of fixation plays a critical role in perceived duration. The duration of an interval located in the upper visual field is perceived as longer when participants fixate the higher visual source and shorter when the fixation point is set in the middle; this latter result also generally applies when the fixation point is in the lower source. Finally, for the sensitivity level, there was a significant segment (upper versus lower) × direction (descending versus ascending) interaction in experiment 1; a similar interaction effect varied according to the fixation point in experiment 2. In experiment 2, the Weber fractions were around 0.22. Most results can be explained in terms of the need to shift attention from one visual source—for marking time intervals—to another.


2018 ◽  
Vol 84 (4) ◽  
pp. 915-931 ◽  
Author(s):  
Elisa Kim Fromboluti ◽  
J. Devin McAuley
Keyword(s):  

2005 ◽  
Vol 94 (4) ◽  
pp. 2263-2274 ◽  
Author(s):  
Jiping Zhang ◽  
Kyle T. Nakamoto ◽  
Leonard M. Kitzes

Sounds commonly occur in sequences, such as in speech. It is therefore important to understand how the occurrence of one sound affects the response to a subsequent sound. We approached this question by determining how a conditioning stimulus alters the response areas of single neurons in the primary auditory cortex (AI) of barbiturate-anesthetized cats. The response areas consisted of responses to stimuli that varied in level at the two ears and delivered at the characteristic frequency of each cell. A binaural conditioning stimulus was then presented ≥50 ms before each of the stimuli comprising the level response area. An effective preceding stimulus alters the shape and severely reduces the size and response magnitude of the level response area. This ability of the preceding stimulus depends on its proximity in the level domain to the level response area, not on its absolute level or on the size of the response it evokes. Preceding stimuli evoke a nonlinear inhibition across the level response area that results in an increased selectivity of a cortical neuron for its preferred binaural stimuli. The selectivity of AI neurons during the processing of a stream of acoustic stimuli is likely to be restricted to a portion of their level response areas apparent in the tone-alone condition. Thus rather than being static, level response areas are fluid; they can vary greatly in extent, shape and response magnitude. The dynamic modulation of the level response area and level selectivity of AI neurons might be related to several tasks confronting the central auditory system.


2019 ◽  
Vol 19 (10) ◽  
pp. 210c
Author(s):  
Chris Paffen ◽  
Jim Maarseveen ◽  
Frans AJ Verstraten ◽  
Hinze Hogendoorn
Keyword(s):  

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document