Orientation and dispersal of hatchling Blanding’s turtles (Emydoidea blandingii) from experimental nests

2009 ◽  
Vol 87 (9) ◽  
pp. 755-766 ◽  
Author(s):  
M. J. Pappas ◽  
J. D. Congdon ◽  
B. J. Brecke ◽  
J. D. Capps

We determined initial dispersal directions of 1052 naïve and 278 experienced hatchling Blanding’s turtles (Emydoidea blandingii (Holbrook, 1838)) in experimental arenas in a variety of settings. Dispersal of naïve hatchlings was nonrandom in 7 of 10 sites. All nonrandom dispersal patterns suggested hatchlings primarily used vision to orient toward dark far horizons, particularly those associated with riparian habitats. We found no evidence that hatchlings use positive geotaxis, olfaction, humidity gradients, or scent trailing of other individuals during dispersal. Despite the lack of relationships between the changing position of the sun and relationships between nest sites and wetlands, patterns of dispersal were different for hatchlings released in the morning and late afternoon at two sites. Comparisons of the dispersal of naïve and translocated experienced hatchlings (those with previous exposure to environmental cues) suggest that hatchlings develop a sun compass within 2 days of emergence from nests. Based on all nonrandom dispersals of hatchlings at arenas, the estimated maximum perception distance of hatchlings was 325 m. In some situations, forest succession, agriculture activities, and introduction of pine trees may increase risks faced by hatchlings dispersing from nests by reducing their ability to find wetlands.

2019 ◽  
Vol 286 (1907) ◽  
pp. 20190934 ◽  
Author(s):  
Haneul Jang ◽  
Christophe Boesch ◽  
Roger Mundry ◽  
Vidrich Kandza ◽  
Karline R. L. Janmaat

The ability to know the direction of food sources is important for the foraging success of hunter–gatherers, especially in rainforests where dense vegetation limits visual detection distances. Besides sex and age, prior experience with the environment and the use of environmental cues are known to influence orientation abilities of humans. Among environmental cues, the position of the sun in the sky is important for orientation of diurnal animal species. However, whether or to what extent humans use the sun is largely unknown. Here, we investigated orientation abilities of the Mbendjele BaYaka people in the Republic of Congo, by conducting pointing tests ( N participants = 54, age: 6–76 years) in different locations in the rainforest. The Mbendjele were overall highly accurate at pointing to out-of-sight targets (median error: 6°). Pointing accuracy increased with age, but sex did not affect accuracy. Crucially, sun visibility increased pointing accuracy in young participants, especially when they were far from the camp. However, this effect became less apparent in older participants who exhibited high pointing accuracy, also when the sun was not visible. This study extends our understandings of orientation abilities of human foragers and provides the first behavioural evidence for sun compass use in humans.


2000 ◽  
Vol 179 ◽  
pp. 193-196
Author(s):  
V. I. Makarov ◽  
A. G. Tlatov

AbstractA possible scenario of polar magnetic field reversal of the Sun during the Maunder Minimum (1645–1715) is discussed using data of magnetic field reversals of the Sun for 1880–1991 and the14Ccontent variations in the bi-annual rings of the pine-trees in 1600–1730 yrs.


1930 ◽  
Vol 142 (3) ◽  
pp. 232-234
Author(s):  
Alexander Klemin
Keyword(s):  
The Sun ◽  

Author(s):  
Michael V. Metz

The strike days had a playful spirit, but in the evenings student mood darkened; they threw rocks, broke windows; violence spread. On Saturday rain was predicted but instead the sun shone and a peaceful Illiac rally drew thousands; however, with a late afternoon march to the building site, the mood changed and trouble emerged. Crowds took over Green Street, police and guardsmen cleared it; state troopers arrived, pushing, shoving, and making arbitrary arrests. The crowd grew angry, and soon mayhem reigned across the campus--but then the rain came, the crowds dispersed, and the violence ended.


Author(s):  
Dwi Putra Jaya
Keyword(s):  
The Sun ◽  

Abstract: To know the direction of Qiblah is already a lot of tools. Can be through measurements, can also use a prayer rug that has a compass Qiblah direction that is widely used in mosques. However, in order to attain the virtue of charity, it is necessary to make sure that the direction approached in the direction precisely facing the Temple. The way of determining the direction of the Qiblah for mosques has evolved in accordance with the development of knowledge held by Islamic societies, in the early stages using a very simple way, then progressed by using tools to measure it, among them Trigonometry, the shadow of the sun, compass magnet, transparent compass, compass Qibla, protractor, Rubu ‘mujayyab, string or thread, stick istiwa’ and waterpas, lot, elbow.


1990 ◽  
Vol 45 (6) ◽  
pp. 723-726 ◽  
Author(s):  
Randolf Menzel ◽  
Lars Chittka ◽  
Stefan Eichmüller ◽  
Karl Geiger ◽  
Dagmar Peitsch ◽  
...  

Abstract A recent model of landmark orientation by the bee assumes that the memory of the landmarks is arranged in a kind of a mental map. Our experiments disprove this assumption and show that the sun compass dominates the orientation without any indication of mental operations within a map-like representation of landmarks or of compass vectors and distances.


2020 ◽  
Vol 19 (1) ◽  
pp. 67
Author(s):  
Emma M. Buckardt ◽  
Gary A. Glowacki ◽  
James P. Gibbs

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document