Teacher Suspiciousness of Experimenter's Intent and the Meditation of Teacher Expectancy Effects

1982 ◽  
Vol 3 (3) ◽  
pp. 219-230
Author(s):  
Sara E. Snodgrass ◽  
Robert Rosenthal
1984 ◽  
Vol 6 (4) ◽  
pp. 408-429 ◽  
Author(s):  
Thomas J. Martinek ◽  
William B. Karper

The purpose of this study was to describe the operation of teacher expectancy effects within two instructional climates of elementary physical education classes. Specifically, high and low expectancy groups were compared during noncompetitive and competitive instruction in terms of teacher-student interaction and perceived expression of effort. Four alternating experimental phases of instruction were employed. Analysis of the interaction data revealed that low expectancy students received significantly more praise and encouragement during the first (noncompetitive) phase and the fourth (competitive) phase than did high expectancy students. They also received significantly more empathy from their teachers during both competitive phases of instruction. High expectancy students were perceived to exhibit significantly more effort than low expectancy students during all four phases.


2018 ◽  
Vol 49 (1) ◽  
pp. 57-90 ◽  
Author(s):  
Faiza M. Jamil ◽  
Ross A. Larsen ◽  
Bridget K. Hamre

The current study uses a large, nationally representative data set and a new method for computing teacher expectations to better understand the developmental effect of mathematics teacher expectations on future student achievement. The study utilizes autoregressive cross-lagged models with 5 time points between kindergarten and 8th grade as well as multigroup modeling to examine group differences in teacher expectancy effects on achievement for girls and minority students. Results indicate that students' experiences with teacher expectations from 1 time point to the next are not significantly associated with one another, but their association with future student achievement grows over time. Teacher expectancy effects in mathematics are stronger for White girls, minority girls, and minority boys than they are for White boys. Implications for teaching are discussed.


2018 ◽  
Vol 49 (1) ◽  
pp. 111-115 ◽  
Author(s):  
Faiza M. Jamil

I appreciate the opportunity to respond to the thoughtful comments made by Alan Schoenfeld (2018) and Jon Star (2018) in their commentaries on replication studies in this issue of JRME, including their comments on our study of teacher expectancy effects (Jamil, Larsen, & Hamre, 2018). I have decided to write this rejoinder in the form of a personal reflection. As academics, we carry the tremendous burden of expertise, and perhaps that is partly why, as pointed out by Schoenfeld (2018), the academic reward system focuses so heavily on novelty and innovation. With our expertise, we are supposed to have all the answers, solve all the problems, and do so in brilliant, new ways. Replication studies are undervalued because they not only, by definition, recreate past research but, perhaps, also bring into question another scholar‧s expertise. Star (2018) even states that one of the three criteria of an outstanding replication study is that it “convincingly shows that there is reason to believe that the results of the original study may be flawed” (p. 99). Although this rigorous examination is precisely the way to build trust in the quality of our findings and move the field forward, it is also what makes it challenging to have candid conversations about what we do not know.


2015 ◽  
Vol 41 ◽  
pp. 1-12 ◽  
Author(s):  
Alena Friedrich ◽  
Barbara Flunger ◽  
Benjamin Nagengast ◽  
Kathrin Jonkmann ◽  
Ulrich Trautwein

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document