scholarly journals Perceiving Perpendicular and Parallel Contours in the Frontoparallel Plane

2018 ◽  
Author(s):  
Ekaterina Koshmanova ◽  
Tadamasa Sawada
Keyword(s):  
Perception ◽  
1995 ◽  
Vol 24 (9) ◽  
pp. 995-1010 ◽  
Author(s):  
Emiel Reith ◽  
Chang Hong Liu

Adult subjects drew the visual projection of two models. One model was a trapezoid placed in the frontoparallel plane. The other was a tilted rectangle which displayed the same projective shape on a frontoparallel plane as the trapezoid. The drawing conditions were varied in two ways: the model remained available for inspection during the drawing task or it was masked after initial inspection; the subjects drew on paper placed flat on the table or on a vertical glass pane placed in front of the model (ie on a da Vinci window). The results were that (i) the projective shape of the frontoparallel trapezoid was reproduced accurately whereas that of the tilted rectangle was systematically distorted in the direction of its actual physical dimensions; (ii) when subjects drew on paper, the presence or absence of a view of the model made no difference to the amount of distortion; (iii) drawing on a da Vinci window improved accuracy even when the model was hidden. These findings provide information about the relative roles of object-centred knowledge, perceptual abilities, and depiction skills in drawing performance.


Perception ◽  
1989 ◽  
Vol 18 (2) ◽  
pp. 237-242 ◽  
Author(s):  
P Lánský ◽  
Naum Yakimoff ◽  
T Radil ◽  
L Mitrani

The error in estimating the orientation of a dot pattern was measured as the difference between the orientation of the least-squared-distances line (LS-line) of the pattern and the orientation of a line adjusted by the subject to match the perceived orientation of the pattern. Analysis of the mean errors (averaged over ten subjects) obtained for one hundred patterns confirmed that the orientation of the LS-line represents the orientation of elongated dot-patterns. It is shown that estimated orientation was systematically biased towards the nearest 45° oblique meridian. This bias points to the importance of the ±45° directions as natural norms for left- and right-side tilt in the frontoparallel plane.


2010 ◽  
Vol 1 (3) ◽  
pp. 324-324
Author(s):  
V. Cornilleau-Peres ◽  
L.C. Tai ◽  
L. -F. Cheong

1992 ◽  
Vol 69 (7) ◽  
pp. 544-549 ◽  
Author(s):  
ARNULF REMOLE ◽  
STEPHEN M. CODE ◽  
CYNTHIA E. MATYAS ◽  
MURRAY A. McLEOD ◽  
CONNIE K. TO

Perception ◽  
1994 ◽  
Vol 23 (9) ◽  
pp. 1037-1048 ◽  
Author(s):  
Sachio Nakamizo ◽  
Koichi Shimono ◽  
Michiaki Kondo ◽  
Hiroshi Ono

Visual directions of the two stimuli in Panum's limiting case with different interstimulus and convergence distances confirmed the predictions from the reformulated Wells—Hering's laws of visual direction. In experiment 1, six observers each converged on the midpoint of the interstimulus axis at 30, 60, and 90 cm from the eyes and adjusted a probe on the fixation plane to be in the same visual direction as that of each stimulus. Visual direction of the far stimulus was always nonveridical whereas that of the near stimulus was veridical only when its retinal disparity was small. In experiment 2, three observers each converged on the intersection of mid-sagittal plane and (a) the frontoparallel plane of the near stimulus, (b) that of the midpoint between the two stimuli, or (c) that of the far stimulus. The midpoint of the interstimulus axis was 60 cm from the eyes. Visual direction of the far stimulus was veridical only with convergence at the far plane. Visual direction of the near stimulus was veridical with convergence at the near plane, and also, only when its retinal disparity was small, with convergence at the two other planes.


Perception ◽  
1997 ◽  
Vol 26 (1) ◽  
pp. 29-38 ◽  
Author(s):  
Jukka Häkkinen ◽  
Göte Nyman

In binocular vision horizontal magnification of one retinal image leads to a percept of three-dimensional slant around a vertical axis. It is demonstrated that the perception of slant is diminished when an occlusion interpretation is possible. A frontoparallel plane located in the immediate vicinity of a slanted surface in a location which allows a perception of occlusion reduces the magnitude of perceived slant significantly. When the same plane is placed on the other side, the slant perception is normal because there is no alternative occlusion interpretation. The results indicate that a common border between the occluder and a slanted surface is not a necessary condition for the reduction effect. If the edges are displaced and the edge of the slanted surface is placed in a location in which it could be occluded, the effect still appears.


1967 ◽  
Vol 25 (2) ◽  
pp. 621-628 ◽  
Author(s):  
Roy B. Mefferd ◽  
Betty A. Wieland

10 Os viewed under extreme reduction conditions 5 simple, plane, featureless figures and an Ames trapezoidal window as they rotated slowly (5 rpm) in the frontoparallel plane. Judgments of shape, slant, and type of movement were obtained for 4-min. periods first with binocular regard and later with monocular. As more perspective cues were introduced and as viewing time increased, there were progressive increases in depth indicants with all three types of judgments.


Perception ◽  
10.1068/p5641 ◽  
2007 ◽  
Vol 36 (7) ◽  
pp. 980-989 ◽  
Author(s):  
J Farley Norman ◽  
Elizabeth Y Wiesemann ◽  
Hideko F Norman ◽  
M Jett Taylor ◽  
Warren D Craft

The sensitivity of observers to nonrigid bending was evaluated in two experiments. In both experiments, observers were required to discriminate on any given trial which of two bending rods was more elastic. In experiment 1, both rods bent within the same oriented plane, and bent either in a frontoparallel plane or bent in depth. In experiment 2, the two rods within any given trial bent in different, randomly chosen orientations in depth. The results of both experiments revealed that human observers are sensitive to, and can reliably detect, relatively small differences in bending (the average Weber fraction across experiments 1 and 2 was 9.0%). The performance of the human observers was compared to that of models that based their elasticity judgments upon either static projected curvature or mean and maximal projected speed. Despite the fact that all of the observers reported compelling 3-D perceptions of bending in depth, their judgments were both qualitatively and quantitatively consistent with the performance of the models. This similarity suggests that relatively straightforward information about the elasticity of simple bending objects is available in projected retinal images.


Perception ◽  
1987 ◽  
Vol 16 (2) ◽  
pp. 187-192 ◽  
Author(s):  
Paola Bressan ◽  
Giorgio Vallortigara

The Saturn illusion is a stereokinetic effect that occurs when a flat pattern composed of a full ellipse with two symmetrical semirings is rotated slowly in the frontoparallel plane. Subjects report seeing an egg-shaped object inserted into a circular ring, and the two objects move solidly into 3-D space as a single rigid body. Inexperienced observers show a conspicuous delay before reaching this percept. Two experiments are reported in which it is shown that this incubation time progressively decreases with repeated exposures to the stimulus pattern. A certain amount of time (14 s on average) is, however, required to obtain the effect, even after six successive exposures. It is argued that this time, which is independent of the speed of rotation and is not further reducible, is a fixed entity and is needed to compute the most rigid 3-D solution from deformations in the 2-D image. The results are discussed in relation to current theories of perception of structure from motion.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document