poggendorff illusion
Recently Published Documents


TOTAL DOCUMENTS

114
(FIVE YEARS 4)

H-INDEX

19
(FIVE YEARS 0)

i-Perception ◽  
2021 ◽  
Vol 12 (3) ◽  
pp. 204166952110156
Author(s):  
Stuart Anstis ◽  
Patrick Cavanagh

A moving frame can dramatically displace the perceived location of stimuli flashed before and after the motion. Here, we use a moving frame to rearrange flashed elements into the form of classic illusions. Without the moving frame, the initial arrangement of the flashed elements has no illusory effect. The question is whether the frame-induced displacement of position precedes or follows the processes underlying the illusions. This illusory offset of flashed chevrons does generate a Müller-Lyer illusion and the illusory offset of two line segments does create a Poggendorff illusion. We conclude that the site where the frame-induced position shift emerges must precede the site at which the Müller-Lyer and Poggendorf illusions arise.


2020 ◽  
Vol 46 (4) ◽  
pp. 366-372
Author(s):  
S. I. Rychkova ◽  
R. I. Sandimirov ◽  
L. V. Kosobutskaya

Author(s):  
Kai Hamburger ◽  
Thorsten Hansen ◽  
Karl R. Gegenfurtner

This chapter briefly introduces nine classical geometric-optical illusions. These include the Delboeuf illusion, the Ebbinghaus illusion, the Judd illusion, the Müller-Lyer illusion, the Ponzo illusion, the vertical illusion, the Hering illusion, the Poggendorff illusion, and the Zoellner illusion. It then demonstrates that they persist under different luminance conditions and under isoluminance. The empirical findings show that our conscious percept is similarly affected by luminance conditions and isoluminance, suggesting that joint contour processing (chromatic and luminance) may extend well beyond early visual areas. The chapter further discusses these concepts in terms of the magnocellular system, the parvocellular system, and the koniocellular system.


Perception ◽  
2016 ◽  
Vol 46 (1) ◽  
pp. 78-89 ◽  
Author(s):  
Sergey I. Bozhevolnyi

The Poggendorff illusion in its classical form of parallel lines interrupting a transversal is viewed from the perspective of being related to the everyday experience of observing the light refraction in water. It is argued that if one considers a transversal to be a light ray in air and the parallel lines to form an occluding strip of a medium with the refractive index being between that of air and water, then one should be able to account, both qualitatively and quantitatively, for most of the features associated with the Poggendorff illusion. Statistical treatment of the visual experiments conducted with seven participants, each analyzing 50 configurations having different intercepting angles and strip widths, resulted in the effective refractive index of the occluding strip N = 1.13 ± 0.15, which is sufficiently close to the average (between that of water and air) refractive index of ∼1.17. It is further argued that the same mechanism can also be employed to account for many variants of the Poggendorff illusion, including the corner-Poggendorff pattern, as well as for the Hering illusion.


Perception ◽  
10.1068/p7788 ◽  
2015 ◽  
Vol 44 (4) ◽  
pp. 383-399 ◽  
Author(s):  
Daniele Zavagno ◽  
Olga Daneyko ◽  
Natale Stucchi
Keyword(s):  

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document