autostereoscopic display
Recently Published Documents


TOTAL DOCUMENTS

310
(FIVE YEARS 42)

H-INDEX

18
(FIVE YEARS 1)

2021 ◽  
Vol 60 (12) ◽  
Author(s):  
Tomoi Nishimura ◽  
Goro Hamagishi ◽  
Kaoru Kusafuka ◽  
Kayo Yoshimoto ◽  
Hideya Takahashi

Author(s):  
Che-Hao Hsu ◽  
Yi-Leh Wu ◽  
Wen-Huang Cheng ◽  
Yu-Jen Chen ◽  
Kai-Lung Hua

Author(s):  
Ruiyang Li ◽  
Tianqi Huang ◽  
Hanying Liang ◽  
Boxuan Han ◽  
Xinran Zhang ◽  
...  

2021 ◽  
Vol 52 (S2) ◽  
pp. 462-467
Author(s):  
Nan Zhang ◽  
Tianqi Huang ◽  
Yang Meng ◽  
Xinran Zhang ◽  
Hongen Liao

2021 ◽  
Vol 52 (S2) ◽  
pp. 26-29
Author(s):  
Ruiyang Li ◽  
Tianqi Huang ◽  
Xinran Zhang ◽  
Hongen Liao

2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Luca Lo Verde ◽  
Anthony Matthew Norcia

When fixating an object, observers typically under or over-converge by a small amount, a phenomenon known as "fixation disparity". Fixation disparity is typically measured with physical fixation targets and dichotically presented nonius lines. Here we made fixation disparity measurements with an autostereoscopic display, varying the retinal eccentricity and disparity of the fixation targets. Measurements were made in a group of four practiced observers and in a group of thirteen experimentally naïve observers. Fixation disparities with a zero-disparity target were in the direction of fixation behind the plane of the screen and the magnitude of the fixation disparity grew with the eccentricity of the fixation targets (1-5 deg in the practiced observers and 1 – 10 deg in the naïve observers). Fixation disparity also increased with increasing disparity of the targets, especially when they were presented at crossed disparities. Fixation disparities were larger overall for naïve observers who additionally did not converge in front of the screen when vergence demand was created by crossed disparity fusion locks presented at 5 and 10 deg eccentricities.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Neil Dodgson

A head-tracked display could be made from a two-view autostereoscopic display where head-tracking allows the display to swap the two views when the eyes move from viewing zone to viewing zone. Variations in human interpupillary distance mean that this basic two-view version will not work well for the significant minority of the population who have interpupillary distance significantly different from the average. Woodgate et al. proposed, in 1997, that a three-view system would work well. Analysis of an ideal version of their proposal shows that it does work well for the vast majority of the population. However, most multi-view, multi-lobe autostereoscopic displays have drawbacks which mean that, in practice, such a system would be unacceptable because of the inter-view dark zones generated by the inter-pixel dark zones on the underlying display technology. Variations of such displays have been developed which remove the inter-view dark zones by allowing adjacent views to overlap with one another: the views appear to smoothly blend from one to the next at the expense of a little blurring. Such displays need at least five viewing zones to accommodate the majority of the adult population with head-tracking and at least six viewing zones to accommodate everyone. © 2006 SPIE-IS&T.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Neil Dodgson

A head-tracked display could be made from a two-view autostereoscopic display where head-tracking allows the display to swap the two views when the eyes move from viewing zone to viewing zone. Variations in human interpupillary distance mean that this basic two-view version will not work well for the significant minority of the population who have interpupillary distance significantly different from the average. Woodgate et al. proposed, in 1997, that a three-view system would work well. Analysis of an ideal version of their proposal shows that it does work well for the vast majority of the population. However, most multi-view, multi-lobe autostereoscopic displays have drawbacks which mean that, in practice, such a system would be unacceptable because of the inter-view dark zones generated by the inter-pixel dark zones on the underlying display technology. Variations of such displays have been developed which remove the inter-view dark zones by allowing adjacent views to overlap with one another: the views appear to smoothly blend from one to the next at the expense of a little blurring. Such displays need at least five viewing zones to accommodate the majority of the adult population with head-tracking and at least six viewing zones to accommodate everyone. © 2006 SPIE-IS&T.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document