Anthropogenic noise and atmospheric absorption of sound induce amplitude shifts in the songs of Southern House Wren (Troglodytes aedon musculus)

2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Guilherme Sementili-Cardoso ◽  
Reginaldo José Donatelli
2001 ◽  
Vol 79 (8) ◽  
pp. 1527-1531 ◽  
Author(s):  
L. Scott Johnson ◽  
Jennifer E. Leyhe ◽  
Caryn Werner
Keyword(s):  

Ethology ◽  
2004 ◽  
Vol 110 (9) ◽  
pp. 693-705 ◽  
Author(s):  
L. Scott Johnson ◽  
Robin L. Rauch ◽  
Sara N. Dellone

2019 ◽  
Vol 97 (1) ◽  
pp. 50-56
Author(s):  
C.L. Gable ◽  
T.J. Underwood ◽  
G.P. Setliff

House Wrens (Troglodytes aedon Vieillot, 1809) regularly add spider egg cases (Arachnida: Araneae) to their nests, which may be an example of heterospecific cleaning. This behavior involves one animal employing another to remove parasites from their nests. In House Wren nests, juvenile spiders hatching from egg cases may facilitate the reduction of mites that feed on nestlings. We tested this ectoparasite reduction hypothesis by monitoring House Wren nests for spider egg cases and by collecting completed nests to compare the number of spider egg cases and Dermanyssus hirundinis (Hermann, 1804) mites. No significant relationship was found between the number of spider egg cases and number of D. hirundinis mites in nests. We also found no significant relationship between the number of D. hirundinis mites in nests and the body condition of nestlings. Finally, no significant difference was found between the number of D. hirundinis mites in early versus late season nests, but significantly more spider egg cases were added to late season nests. Of a subsample of spider egg cases dissected, we found that 28% contained spider eggs or embryos. We also identified three species of juvenile spiders from House Wren nests using DNA barcoding. Overall, we found no evidence that spider egg cases reduce the number of D. hirundinis mites or engender better quality offspring in House Wren nests.


The Condor ◽  
2001 ◽  
Vol 103 (3) ◽  
pp. 587-591 ◽  
Author(s):  
Lisa A. Ellis ◽  
John D. Styrsky ◽  
Robert C. Dobbs ◽  
Charles F. Thompson

Abstract The degree of hatching synchrony in clutches of passerine birds frequently varies among species and among individuals of the same species. Many hypotheses have been proposed to explain why some eggs hatch several days after others in a clutch. We tested one of these hypotheses, the energetic-constraints hypothesis, which proposes that females in poor physical condition postpone initiating incubation and hatch their clutches synchronously, whereas females in good condition begin incubation early and hatch their clutches asynchronously. We tested the hypothesis using the House Wren (Troglodytes aedon) because recent studies have found little difference in productivity between synchronously and asynchronously hatching clutches in this species, suggesting that the degree of hatching synchrony varies for reasons unrelated to nestling growth and survival. We used logistic regression to test the dependence of the degree of hatching synchrony on each of two measures of female condition. We found no relationship between female condition and degree of hatching synchrony. These results and two other lines of evidence are inconsistent with the energetic-constraints hypothesis as an explanation for variation in degree of hatching synchrony in this House Wren population. ¿Predice la Condición de las Hembras la Sincronía de Eclosión en Troglodytes aedon? Resumen. Con frecuencia, el grado de sincronía en la eclosión de nidadas de aves paserinas varía entre especies y entre individuos de la misma especie. Se han propuesto muchas hipótesis para explicar por qué algunos huevos eclosionan varios días después que otros en una misma nidada. Pusimos a prueba una de esas hipótesis, la de las limitaciones energéticas, que propone que hembras en mala condición física postponen el inicio de la incubación y sus nidadas eclosionan sincrónicamente, mientras que aquellas en buena condición inician la incubación temprano y sus nidadas eclosionan asincrónicamente. Pusimos a prueba la hipótesis usando a Troglodytes aedon, pues estudios recientes en esta especie han encontrado pocas diferencias en productividad entre nidadas de eclosión sincrónica y asincrónica, lo que sugiere que el grado de sincronía en la eclosión varía por motivos no relacionados con el crecimiento y supervivencia de los pichones. Utilizamos regresión logística para evaluar el grado de dependencia de la sincronía de eclosión en dos medidas de condición física de las hembras. No encontramos ninguna relación entre la condición de las hembras y el grado de sincronía en la eclosión. Estos resultados y otras dos líneas de evidencia son inconsistentes con la hipótesis de las limitaciones energéticas como una explicación para la variación en el grado de sincronía de eclosión en esta población de T. aedon.


The Auk ◽  
1934 ◽  
Vol 51 (1) ◽  
pp. 87-88 ◽  
Author(s):  
T. Gilbert Pearson

1997 ◽  
Vol 75 (2) ◽  
pp. 302-307 ◽  
Author(s):  
Stanislav Pribil ◽  
Jaroslav Picman

We tested five hypotheses that may explain why House Wren (Troglodytes aedon) nests are rarely parasitized by Brown-headed Cowbirds (Molothrus ater). House Wrens may prevent parasitism in five ways: (1) by choosing to nest in cavities with small entrances (inaccessible-entrance hypothesis), (2) by restricting the size of the entrance with nest material (nest-structure hypothesis), (3) by puncturing and ejecting parasitic eggs (puncture–ejection hypothesis), (4) by burying the parasitized clutch under a new nest (egg-burial hypothesis), or (5) by abandoning the parasitized nest altogether (nest-desertion hypothesis). We tested these hypotheses in field experiments and found that (i) female cowbirds cannot enter circular entrances smaller than 38 mm in diameter, (ii) wrens prefer cavities with small entrances (inaccessible to cowbirds) to those with large entrances (accessible to cowbirds), (iii) when forced to breed in cavities with large entrances, wrens do not reduce the entrance size with nest material, (iv) despite the unusual strength of cowbird eggs, wrens are physically capable of puncture–ejecting them, (v) wrens do not puncture–eject cowbird eggs from their own nests, (vi) wrens do not abandon parasitized nests or bury the parasitized clutches under new nests. These results are consistent with the inaccessible-entrance hypothesis. We propose that additional nesting adaptations, as well as active cowbird avoidance of House Wrens, may contribute to the low frequency of cowbird parasitism.


The Auk ◽  
1928 ◽  
Vol 45 (2) ◽  
pp. 186-199 ◽  
Author(s):  
S. Prentiss Baldwin ◽  
W. Wedgwood Bowen

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document