Neural basis of the visual working memory deficit in schizophrenia: Merging evidence from fMRI and EEG

2021 ◽  
Vol 236 ◽  
pp. 61-68
Author(s):  
Molly A. Erickson ◽  
Britta Hahn ◽  
John E. Kiat ◽  
Luz Maria Alliende ◽  
Steven J. Luck ◽  
...  
2014 ◽  
Vol 14 (10) ◽  
pp. 171-171 ◽  
Author(s):  
S. Wijeakumar ◽  
V. Magnotta ◽  
A. Buss ◽  
J. Spencer

2018 ◽  
Vol 18 (10) ◽  
pp. 116
Author(s):  
Kartik Sreenivasan ◽  
Ainsley Temudo ◽  
Vahan Babushkin

2009 ◽  
Vol 24 (S1) ◽  
pp. 1-1
Author(s):  
P. Boyer

Since long working memory deficit has been considered as one of the major cognitive dysfunction encountered in schizophrenia. Working memory is critical for human reasoning, judgment and decision and depends upon the integrity of prefrontal and cingulate circuitry. As a consequence coupling pharmacological (amphetamine sensitization, subchronic phencyclidine administration, neurodevelopmental insult) and behavioral approaches of prefrontal functions in animals seems to be a cue totally appropriate for elucidating the mechanisms of this dysfunction in man (Castner,2004).In rodent models aberrant dopaminergic and glutamatergic signaling in medial prefrontal cortex has undoubtedly an impact on memory and learning, But the analogy between these deficits and a true working memory deficit in man is not obvious. Due to the higher degree of homology between human and non-human primates behavioral tests in apes seem to be more relevant but once again they can not be considered as reflecting strictly the consequences of working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. Another approach is to extend the insights gained from the study of normal brain organization in animal models to better understand the neural basis on which working memory functions are based (Tanaka, 2006). Dissecting the cellular and circuit basis of prefrontal and cingulate cortices can give an idea how direct and indirect intercellular mechanisms are modulating working memory Nevertheless the behavioral part of this type of study remains non conclusive. It seems in fact that working memory itself is probably not a good candidate and that contextual-binding tasks (which explore encoding and retrieval) are more appropriate (Boyer,2007).


NeuroImage ◽  
2010 ◽  
Vol 49 (4) ◽  
pp. 3394-3403 ◽  
Author(s):  
Sachiko Takahama ◽  
Satoru Miyauchi ◽  
Jun Saiki

2021 ◽  
Vol 11 (1) ◽  
Author(s):  
Ilja Wagner ◽  
Christian Wolf ◽  
Alexander C. Schütz

AbstractMotor adaptation maintains movement accuracy over the lifetime. Saccadic eye movements have been used successfully to study the mechanisms and neural basis of adaptation. Using behaviorally irrelevant targets, it has been shown that saccade adaptation is driven by errors only in a brief temporal interval after movement completion. However, under natural conditions, eye movements are used to extract information from behaviorally relevant objects and to guide actions manipulating these objects. In this case, the action outcome often becomes apparent only long after movement completion, outside the supposed temporal window of error evaluation. Here, we show that saccade adaptation can be driven by error signals long after the movement when using behaviorally relevant targets. Adaptation occurred when a task-relevant target appeared two seconds after the saccade, or when a retro-cue indicated which of two targets, stored in visual working memory, was task-relevant. Our results emphasize the important role of visual working memory for optimal movement control.


NeuroImage ◽  
1998 ◽  
Vol 7 (4) ◽  
pp. S870
Author(s):  
D.S. Lee ◽  
J.S. Lee ◽  
S-K. Lee ◽  
H. Nam ◽  
S.K. Kim ◽  
...  

NeuroImage ◽  
2014 ◽  
Vol 91 ◽  
pp. 336-343 ◽  
Author(s):  
Agneish Dutta ◽  
Kushal Shah ◽  
Juha Silvanto ◽  
David Soto

NeuroImage ◽  
2021 ◽  
pp. 118650
Author(s):  
Petra Hermann ◽  
Béla Weiss ◽  
Balázs Knakker ◽  
Petra Madurka ◽  
Annamária Manga ◽  
...  

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document