scholarly journals Functional Discourse Grammar

Linguistics ◽  
2019 ◽  
Author(s):  
Inge Genee

Functional Discourse Grammar (FDG) is an expanded version of the Functional Grammar framework developed by Simon Dik at the University of Amsterdam from the 1970s through the middle of the 1990s. It occupies a middle position in the functional-to-formal continuum: it is functional in being centrally concerned with the effects of pragmatics and semantics on morphosyntactic and phonological form, and it is formal in being interested only in systematic effects on linguistic form and in admitting the existence of arbitrary form where functional explanations fail. FDG is often compared to Role and Reference Grammar and Systemic Functional Linguistics as well as to various cognitive approaches to language. FDG sees itself as responsible for accounting for the linguistic component within a wider model of verbal interaction. The grammar is flanked by components that house those other aspects, including a conceptual component, a contextual component, and an output component. FDG is strongly typologically based in its insistence on investigating the formal and functional limits of human linguistic form. The basic unit of analysis in FDG is the discourse act. All linguistic utterances are analyzed at four separate levels, each of which is internally layered. The interpersonal level deals with the actional aspect of language use, including pragmatics, and accounts for such things as reference, identifiability, illocution, and pragmatic functions such as topic, focus, and contrast. The representational level deals with semantics and accounts for such things as ontological categories (entity types) and distinctions related to tense, aspect, modality, evidentiality, polarity, quantification, qualification, location, manner, valency, semantic functions, and parts-of-speech. The morphosyntactic level deals with morphology and syntax and accounts for such things as word and morpheme order, alignment, dummy insertion, agreement, raising and other displacement phenomena, and the internal structure of words. The phonological level deals with phonology and accounts for such things as prosody, stress, reduplication (to the extent that it is phonological), tone and intonation, syllable structure, and the language’s inventory of phonemes and suprasegmentals. The grammar is flanked by a storehouse often called the fund, which houses primitives that feed the grammatical process at each level. In addition to the lexicon proper, the fund contains the structicon (frames and templates) and the grammaticon (operators). Much recent and current work in FDG concerns itself in one way or another with matters of scope within layers, interactions between levels, interfaces, and mappings between units at different levels or layers. In addition to a descriptively and explanatorily adequate account of specific data, the goal is often to produce generalizations in the forms of hierarchies that produce clear predictions in terms of expected typological patterns, diachronic pathways, and acquisition processes. The author wishes to thank several members of the FDG community for sending crucial references or for assistance with important publications in languages with which she is not familiar, in particular John Connolly, Evelien Keizer, Kees Hengeveld, Lachlan Mackenzie, Hella Olbertz, Thomas Schwaiger, and an anonymous reviewer.

Linguistics ◽  
2016 ◽  
Vol 54 (5) ◽  
Author(s):  
Wim Jansen

AbstractThis study takes Functional Discourse Grammar (FDG) as its theoretical framework, and applies to the phenomena under study the FDG terminology, which may differ from what many readers are used to. Esperanto has an agglutinating morphology. Its words are built on stems which are associated with contentive lexemes in a flexible system of parts of speech. The language has an elaborate stock of lexically dependent morphemes (roots in FDG) for lexeme building. Many basic words are stem-root combinations in Esperanto. The roots applied in them also appear in complex structures, and can be reordered and interchanged easily to produce different complex stems. For this reason, derivation is taken to be hosted in the lexicon. The lexicon must contain a highly developed procedural knowledge component, of which the derivation system is assumed to be a part. Some of the suffixable roots have homonymous variants that define grammatical processes. The combinatorial freedom of roots provides for a lexical expansion tool which is easy to handle, but not without complications. Problematic is the (in)transitivity of lexemes destined to verbal encoding, when applied in an environment that is not naturally theirs. This phenomenon is known to be an important source of mistakes among speakers of the language. It is argued that the lexicon of Esperanto speakers contains paired transitive and intransitive representations of the most current lexemes of this category. The study aims at providing support to better understand the mechanisms at work inside the lexicon and at the interface between the lexicon and the grammar of agglutinating languages.


Pragmatics ◽  
2014 ◽  
Vol 24 (2) ◽  
pp. 349-375 ◽  
Author(s):  
Gareth O’Neill

This paper addresses the systematic influence of contextual factors on the form of linguistic utterances for the different speech channels of communication in Pirahã within the framework of Functional Discourse Grammar (FDG). It is shown that context plays an important role in the choice of a particular speech channel and the associated phonological or phonetic alteration of the underlying linguistic form. A representational system for the description of contextual factors which interact with the grammar and the influence of these contextual factors upon the grammar in Pirahã is then proposed within and elaborating upon the model of FDG. The implications of the speech channels in Pirahã for FDG and for linguistic theory in general are lastly examined.


2008 ◽  
Vol 32 (3) ◽  
pp. 753-785 ◽  
Author(s):  
Kees Hengeveld ◽  
Eva van Lier

In this paper we use the Theory of Functional Discourse Grammar to show that parts-of-speech (PoS) and dependent clauses (DCs) can both be defined in terms of the functions they may express. On the basis of this parallel treatment, we predict that the functional possibilities of PoS and DCs in languages are comparable. This hypothesis is tested using a sample of 23 languages. The results do show similarities between the functional patterns of the two types of constructions.


Pragmatics ◽  
2014 ◽  
Vol 24 (2) ◽  
pp. 297-316 ◽  
Author(s):  
Daniel García Velasco

Functional Discourse Grammar is characterized as the grammatical component of a wider theory of verbal interaction and is linked to two adjacent components: The Conceptual and the Contextual Components. One general property of these components is that they are not open-ended, but are said to contain only that extra-linguistic information which is relevant for the construction and interpretation of the immediate linguistic expression. In this contribution I explore the relation between context and grammar and I conclude that the FDG’s requirement that the Contextual Component should only contain those features which have a systematic impact on grammar is too strict. In particular, I claim that the Contextual Component is relevant in linguistic usage through speakers’ mental representation of its contents, which could be captured in the Conceptual Component. I further argue that the notions of ‘activation’ and ‘sharedness’ are relevant to understanding the motivation of two syntactic processes, subject raising and extraction from NPs, and should therefore find a place in the model even if they do not always lead to systematic effects. It is finally proposed that these pragmatic dimensions could find their way into the grammar by means of unmarked pragmatic configurations or content frames.


2013 ◽  
Vol 5 (1) ◽  
pp. 39-62 ◽  
Author(s):  
Christine Cuskley

This paper provides an overview of the possible function of non-arbitrary mappings between linguistic form and meaning, and presents new empirical evidence showing that shared cross-modal associations may underlie motion sound-symbolism in particular. In terms of function, several lines of empirical and theoretical evidence suggest that non-arbitrary form-meaning connections could have played a crucial role in lexical emergence during language evolution. Furthermore, the persistence of such non-arbitrariness in some areas of modern language may also be highly functional, as recent data has shown that non-arbitrary forms may help to bootstrap learning in children (Imai, Kita, Nagumo, and Okada, 2008) and adults (Nielsen and Rendall, 2012). Given the functional role of these non-arbitrary mappings between linguistic form and meaning, this paper describes new experimental data demonstrating shared mappings between non-sense words and visual motion using a direct matching task. Participants were given nonsense words that varied in terms of their voicing, reduplication, and vowel quality, and asked to change the movement of a ball to match a given word. Results show that back vowels are mapped onto slower speeds, and consonant reduplication with vowel alternation is mapped onto faster speeds. These results show a shared cross-modal association between linguistic sound and motion, which is likely leveraged in sound-symbolic systems found in natural language.


1978 ◽  
Vol 48 ◽  
pp. 31-35
Author(s):  
R. B. Hanson

Several outstanding problems affecting the existing parallaxes should be resolved to form a coherent system for the new General Catalogue proposed by van Altena, as well as to improve luminosity calibrations and other parallax applications. Lutz has reviewed several of these problems, such as: (A) systematic differences between observatories, (B) external error estimates, (C) the absolute zero point, and (D) systematic observational effects (in right ascension, declination, apparent magnitude, etc.). Here we explore the use of cluster and spectroscopic parallaxes, and the distributions of observed parallaxes, to bring new evidence to bear on these classic problems. Several preliminary results have been obtained.


2018 ◽  
Vol 17 (4) ◽  
pp. 193-203 ◽  
Author(s):  
Tanja Hentschel ◽  
Lisa Kristina Horvath ◽  
Claudia Peus ◽  
Sabine Sczesny

Abstract. Entrepreneurship programs often aim at increasing women’s lower entrepreneurial activities. We investigate how advertisements for entrepreneurship programs can be designed to increase women’s application intentions. Results of an experiment with 156 women showed that women indicate (1) lower self-ascribed fit to and interest in the program after viewing a male-typed image (compared to a gender-neutral or female-typed image) in the advertisement; and (2) lower self-ascribed fit to and interest in the program as well as lower application intentions if the German masculine linguistic form of the term “entrepreneur” (compared to the gender-fair word pair “female and male entrepreneur”) is used in the recruitment advertisement. Women’s reactions are most negative when both a male-typed image and the masculine linguistic form appear in the advertisement. Self-ascribed fit and program interest mediate the relationship of advertisement characteristics on application intentions.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document