scholarly journals Titi monkey call sequences vary with predator location and type

2013 ◽  
Vol 9 (5) ◽  
pp. 20130535 ◽  
Author(s):  
Cristiane Cäsar ◽  
Klaus Zuberbühler ◽  
Robert J. Young ◽  
Richard W. Byrne

Animal alarm calls can encode information about a predator's category, size, distance or threat level. In non-human primates, alarm calls typically refer to broad classes of disturbances, in some instances to specific predators. Here, we present the results of a field experiment with a New World primate, the black-fronted titi monkey ( Callicebus nigrifrons ), designed to explore the information conveyed by their alarm call system. Adults produced sequences consisting of two main alarm call types that conveyed, in different parts of the utterance, information about a predator's type and location. In particular, sequence compositions differed depending on whether the predator was a mammalian carnivore or a raptor, and whether it was detected in a tree or on the ground. This is the first demonstration of a sequence-based alarm call system in a non-human animal that has the capacity to encode both location and type of predatory threat.

2019 ◽  
Vol 5 (5) ◽  
pp. eaav3991 ◽  
Author(s):  
Mélissa Berthet ◽  
Geoffrey Mesbahi ◽  
Aude Pajot ◽  
Cristiane Cäsar ◽  
Christof Neumann ◽  
...  

Previous work suggested that titi monkeys Callicebus nigrifrons combine two alarm calls, the A- and B-calls, to communicate about predator type and location. To explore how listeners process these sequences, we recorded alarm call sequences of six free-ranging groups exposed to terrestrial and aerial predator models, placed on the ground or in the canopy, and used multimodel inference to assess the information encoded in the sequences. We then carried out playback experiments to identify the features used by listeners to react to the available information. Results indicated that information about predator type and location were encoded by the proportion of B-call pairs relative to all call pairs of the sequence (i.e., proportion of BB-grams). The results suggest that the meaning of the sequence is not conveyed in a categorical but probabilistic manner. We discuss the implications of these findings for current theories of animal communication and language evolution.


2012 ◽  
Vol 66 (5) ◽  
pp. 653-667 ◽  
Author(s):  
Cristiane Cäsar ◽  
Richard Byrne ◽  
Robert J. Young ◽  
Klaus Zuberbühler

1969 ◽  
Vol 165 (1 Second Confer) ◽  
pp. 360-377 ◽  
Author(s):  
S. H. Boyer ◽  
E. F. Crosby ◽  
G. L. Fuller ◽  
A. N. Noyes ◽  
J. G. Adams

1968 ◽  
Vol 5 (6) ◽  
pp. 538-560 ◽  
Author(s):  
G. E. McKissick ◽  
H. L. Ratcliffe ◽  
A. Koestner

An enzootic of toxoplasmosis occurred in caged squirrel monkeys ( Saimiri sciureus, a New World primate) which resulted in 9 deaths during an interval of 22 months. Diagnosis was based on morphology of the organism, character and distribution of the lesions, and laboratory history of the monkeys. The character of the lesions was essentially necrotic. The chronological incidence of the disease and distribution of lesions and organisms are tabulated. Trypanosoma cruzi which causes Chaga's disease is differentiated morphologically from toxoplasma. Incidence and lesions of the enzootic suggest 5 factors to be considered in the pathogenesis of toxoplasmosis in squirrel monkeys. They are: (1) lack of protective immunity, (2) local concentration of toxin and/or catabolites of reproduction of the organism, (3) individual tissue susceptibility to the organism, (4) capillary thrombosis, and (5) ability of the individual to adapt to its environment.


2016 ◽  
Vol 111 (9) ◽  
pp. 570-576 ◽  
Author(s):  
Gabriela Maíra Pereira de Assis ◽  
Denise Anete Madureira de Alvarenga ◽  
Daniela Camargos Costa ◽  
Júlio César de Souza Junior ◽  
Zelinda Maria Braga Hirano ◽  
...  
Keyword(s):  

2019 ◽  
Vol 22 (6) ◽  
pp. 1149-1157 ◽  
Author(s):  
Jiangping Yu ◽  
Hailin Lu ◽  
Wei Sun ◽  
Wei Liang ◽  
Haitao Wang ◽  
...  

Abstract Species facing similar selection pressures should recognize heterospecific alarm signals. However, no study has so far examined heterospecific alarm-call recognition in response to parasitism by cuckoos. In this study, we tested whether two sympatric host species of the common cuckoo Cuculus canorus, Oriental reed warbler Acrocephalus orientalis (ORW, main host), and black-browed reed warbler Acrocephalus bistrigiceps (BRW, rare host), could recognize each other’s alarm calls in response to cuckoos. Dummies of common cuckoo (parasite) and Eurasian sparrowhawk Accipiter nisus (predator) were used to induce and record alarm calls of the two warbler species, respectively. In the conspecific alarm-call playback experiments, ORW responded more strongly to cuckoo alarm calls than to sparrowhawk alarm calls, while BRW responded less strongly to cuckoo alarm calls than to sparrowhawk alarm calls. In the heterospecific alarm-call playback experiments, both ORW and BRW responded less strongly to cuckoo alarm calls than sparrowhawk alarm calls. BRW seemed to learn the association between parasite-related alarm calls of the ORW and the cuckoo by observing the process of ORW attacking cuckoos. In contrast, alarm calls of BRW to cuckoos were rarely recorded in most cases. BRW with low parasite pressure still developed recognition of heterospecific parasite-related alarm call. Unintended receivers in the same community should recognize heterospecific alarm calls precisely to extract valuable information.


2012 ◽  
Vol 84 (2) ◽  
pp. 405-411 ◽  
Author(s):  
Cristiane Cäsar ◽  
Richard W. Byrne ◽  
William Hoppitt ◽  
Robert J. Young ◽  
Klaus Zuberbühler

1978 ◽  
Vol 75 (2) ◽  
pp. 1004-1008 ◽  
Author(s):  
G. J. Todaro ◽  
C. J. Sherr ◽  
A. Sen ◽  
N. King ◽  
M. D. Daniel ◽  
...  

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document