The Uvular Sounds of Sino-Tibetan

2010 ◽  
Vol 4 (1) ◽  
pp. 165-189
Author(s):  
Zemin Liu

Many languages in the Sino-Tibetan family have uvular sounds. Some scholars have put forward the hypothesis that there should be a set of uvular sounds in Proto-Sino-Tibetan. This paper attempts to evaluate this hypothesis through the following aspects: (1) the synchronic distribution of uvular sounds in modern Sino-Tibetan languages; (2) a review of relevant literature; (3) a typological survey of uvular and velar sounds; (4) physiological and acoustical investigations of uvular sounds; (5) sound changes of uvulars; (6) the origin of the uvulars in Sino-Tibetan languages; (7) areal investigation of Sino-Tibetan uvulars from the perspective of language contact; (8) reconstruction of uvulars in Old Chinese; (9) examples of Sino-Tibetan cognates with uvular sounds.

2021 ◽  
Vol 14 (1) ◽  
pp. 104-138
Author(s):  
Xin Zhao

Abstract This article introduces research on Old Chinese phonology before the Qing dynasty by reviewing the important relevant literature of recent intellectual historians. The article has six parts. The first section is an introduction. The second through fourth parts review the views and arguments over how to understand the works of several important linguists, treating respectively the Southern Song (§ 2, containing Wú Yù 吳棫, Zhū Xī 朱熹 etc.), the Yuan (§ 3, containing Dài Tóng 戴侗, Liú Yùrǔ 劉玉汝, Xióng Pénglái 熊朋來 etc.), and the Ming (§ 4, containing Yáng Shèn 楊慎, Jiāo Hóng 焦竑, Chén Dì 陳第 etc.). Section 5 discusses several methodological issues that concern researchers today, including the historical emergence of three methodologies and methodological issues in researching the history of scholarship. Finally, I point out those topics in need of further investigation.


Linguistics ◽  
2020 ◽  
Vol 58 (1) ◽  
pp. 275-328
Author(s):  
Erik Anonby

AbstractThe complex and cross-linguistically uncommon phonological phenomenon of “emphasis” is best known from Central Semitic languages such as Arabic and Aramaic. It is, however, found to varying degrees in a number of non-Semitic languages in contact with Arabic. This paper describes how in Kumzari, an Indo-European language spoken around the Strait of Hormuz, uvular-pharyngeal emphasis has arisen through language contact and has proliferated through language-internal processes. Beginning with the retention of emphatic consonants in a direct, extensive lexification by Arabic dating back at least 1300 years, emphasis has progressively penetrated the language by means of lexical innovations and two types of sound changes in both borrowed and inherited vocabulary: (i) analogical spread of emphasis onto plain but potentially emphatic consonants; and (ii) a sound change in which z has been invariably recast as an emphatic ẓ with no plain counterpart. The role of the back consonants w, x, q and ḥ, which induce emphasis on potentially emphatic consonants in diachronic processes but not synchronically, highlights the unique way in which this complex phenomenon operates in one non-Semitic language in contact with Arabic.


Diachronica ◽  
2013 ◽  
Vol 30 (1) ◽  
pp. 1-26 ◽  
Author(s):  
Hans-Olav Enger

Traditional views of inflectional changes often run as follows: A morphological opposition expressed by affixes is disturbed by sound changes. It is then left to morphology to ‘clean up the mess’; morphology is merely reactive. If, however, morphology can operate “by itself” (Aronoff 1994, Carstairs-McCarthy 1994, 2001, 2010, Maiden 2004, 2005), one would not expect this classical scenario to be the entire truth. The present paper pursues the hypothesis that there are morphological changes that are not merely reactive, but rather have a morphological motivation. This hypothesis is argued on the basis of well-known reduction processes in Scandinavian case and gender. Both are traditionally taken to represent the classical scenario, but in both, morphology ultimately plays a more prominent role. I argue that neither phonological erosion nor language contact can totally explain Scandinavian case loss and gender reduction. This supports an autonomous role for morphology. Resume On interprete souvent les changements flexionnels comme suit: une opposition morphologique exprimee a l’aide d’affixes se voit modifiee en raison de changements phonetiques. La morphologie, etant par nature purement reactive, doit alors “remettre de l’ordre” dans le systeme. Si, par contre, la morphologie peut agir “seule” (Aronoff 1994, Carstairs-McCarthy 1994, 2001, 2010, Maiden 2004, 2005), il n’est pas interdit de penser que ce scenario classique ne recele pas toute la verite. L’hypothese du present article est qu’il existe des changements morphologiques qui ne sont pas purement reactifs, mais qui sont motives plutot par la morphologie. Cette hypothese se fonde sur les processus bien connus de reduction de cas et de genres dans les langues scandinaves. Dans les deux cas, l’interpretation traditionnelle est qu’il s’agit du scenario classique, alors qu’en fait, la morphologie joue un role plus net. A mon avis, ni l’erosion phonologique ni le contact langagier ne suffisent pour expliquer la perte de cas et la reduction de genres dans les langues scandinaves, ce qui corroborerait l’hypothese du role autonome de la morphologie. Zusammenfassung Traditionellen Auffassungen gemas andert sich die Flexionsmorphologie oft wie folgt: Zuerst fallen Affixe phonologischen Anderungen zum Opfer. Diese Anderungen sind an sich nicht morphologisch, sondern phonologisch motiviert, andern aber die Ausdrucksseite der morphologischen Opposition. Danach muss die Morphologie ‚aufraumen‘. Sie ist also nur ‚reaktiv‘. Wenn aber die Morphologie selbstandig arbeiten kann (‘by itself ’, vgl. z.B. Aronoff 1994, Carstairs-McCarthy 2010, Maiden 2005), ware zu erwarten, dass die traditionelle Auffassung nicht in allen Fallen zutrifft. Im vorliegenden Artikel wird die These vertreten, dass es Entwicklungen gibt, die von der Morphologie selbst ausgelost werden. Diese These wird anhand der bekannten Reduktionsprozesse in der skandinavischen Genus- und Kasusmorphologie verteidigt. Diese Prozesse werden oft als Beispiele fur die traditionelle Auffassung herangezogen. Meines Erachtens kann Kasus- und Genusverlust aber weder durch phonologische ‚Erosion‘ noch durch Sprachkontakt ganz erklart werden. Deswegen muss fur die Morphologie eine autonome Rolle angenommen werden.


Dialectologia ◽  
2020 ◽  
Author(s):  
Bolanle Elizabeth AROKOYO

This study presents a comparative analysis of the phonological systems of the Yorůbá, Owé, Igala and Olůkůmi languages of the Defoid language family of Benue Congo. Data were collected from native speakers using the Ibadan Four Hundred Word List of Basic Items. Using discovered common lexemes in the languages, the classification of the languages sound systems and syllable systems are carried out in order to determine the major patterns of differences and similarities. Some major sound changes were discovered in the lexical items of the languages. The systematic substitutions of sounds also constitute another major finding observed in the languages. It was established in this study that there exists a very strong relationship among these languages. The languages are found to be mutually unintelligible except for Owé that has a degree of mutual intelligibility with Yoruba. The paper concludes that the major reason for divergence is language contact.


2021 ◽  
Vol 2 (1) ◽  
pp. 01-20
Author(s):  
David Natvig ◽  
Joseph Salmons

“Structured heterogeneity”, a founding concept of variationist sociolinguistics, puts focus on the ordered social differentiation in language. We extend the notion of structured heterogeneity to formal phonological structure, i.e., representations based on contrasts, with implications for phonetic implementation. Phonology establishes parameters for what varies and how. Patterns of stability and variability with respect to a given feature’s relationship to representations allow us to ground variationist analysis in a framework that makes predictions about potential sound changes: more structure correlates to more stability; less structure corresponds to more variability. However, even though all change requires variability, not all variability leads to change. Two case studies illustrate this asymmetry, keeping a focus on phonetic change with phonological stability. First, Germanic rhotics (r-sounds) from prehistory to the present day are minimally specified. They show tremendous phonetic variability and change but phonological stability. Second, laryngeal contrasts (voicing or aspiration) vary and change in language contact. We track the accumulation of phonetic change in unspecified members of pairs of the type spelled <s> ≠ <z>, etc. This analysis makes predictions about the regularity of sound change, situating regularity in phonology and irregularity in phonetics and the lexicon. Structured heterogeneity involves the variation inherent within the system for various levels of phonetic and phonological representation. Phonological change, then, is about acquiring or learning different abstract representations based on heterogeneous and variable input.


2021 ◽  
Vol 14 (2) ◽  
pp. 226-245
Author(s):  
Johanna Laakso

The traditional hypothesis of a typological cycle from agglutination via fusion to isolation and back to agglutination, still invoked by many linguists (albeit with caveats and limitations), would imply a natural drift behind typological changes. Accordingly, such typological changes would typically result from internal developments (such as reductive sound changes), while etymological counter-currents (such as segmentable suffixes replacing earlier stem alternations) could rather be due to language contact. On the other hand, the agglutinative type seems to be stable and resistant to typological change especially in Northern Eurasia, and for the change towards a more fusional type, characteristic of some Finnic and Saami languages in the northwestern periphery of Uralic, a contact explanation might seem plausible. However, a closer scrutiny of Estonian, often mentioned as an example of typological change and characteristically impacted by Germanic, shows that in typological change, internal and external motivations intertwine and interact.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document