scholarly journals The size-weight illusion comes along with improved weight discrimination

PLoS ONE ◽  
2020 ◽  
Vol 15 (7) ◽  
pp. e0236440
Author(s):  
Christian Wolf ◽  
Knut Drewing
1970 ◽  
Vol 22 (2) ◽  
pp. 318-328 ◽  
Author(s):  
Helen E. Ross ◽  
R. L. Gregory

The results of several experiments are reported most of which suggest that there is an optimum density for weight discrimination. This density corresponds to the “non-illusory” density, as determined by the density at which a visible weight is correctly matched with a hidden weight. The greater the illusion (whether of heaviness or lightness) the poorer the discrimination. It is pointed out that similar changes in discrimination occur as a result of peripheral sensory adaptation in many modalities; but that the size-weight illusion, and the associated discrimination changes, must be due to a central scaling process. A theoretical model is suggested.


2008 ◽  
Vol 35 (S 01) ◽  
Author(s):  
Y Li ◽  
J Randerath ◽  
G Goldenberg ◽  
J Hermsdörfer

Author(s):  
Akihiro Maehigashi ◽  
Akira Sasada ◽  
Miki Matsumuro ◽  
Fumihisa Shibata ◽  
Asako Kimura ◽  
...  

2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Nina Rohrbach ◽  
Joachim Hermsdörfer ◽  
Lisa-Marie Huber ◽  
Annika Thierfelder ◽  
Gavin Buckingham

AbstractAugmented reality, whereby computer-generated images are overlaid onto the physical environment, is becoming significant part of the world of education and training. Little is known, however, about how these external images are treated by the sensorimotor system of the user – are they fully integrated into the external environmental cues, or largely ignored by low-level perceptual and motor processes? Here, we examined this question in the context of the size–weight illusion (SWI). Thirty-two participants repeatedly lifted and reported the heaviness of two cubes of unequal volume but equal mass in alternation. Half of the participants saw semi-transparent equally sized holographic cubes superimposed onto the physical cubes through a head-mounted display. Fingertip force rates were measured prior to lift-off to determine how the holograms influenced sensorimotor prediction, while verbal reports of heaviness after each lift indicated how the holographic size cues influenced the SWI. As expected, participants who lifted without augmented visual cues lifted the large object at a higher rate of force than the small object on early lifts and experienced a robust SWI across all trials. In contrast, participants who lifted the (apparently equal-sized) augmented cubes used similar force rates for each object. Furthermore, they experienced no SWI during the first lifts of the objects, with a SWI developing over repeated trials. These results indicate that holographic cues initially dominate physical cues and cognitive knowledge, but are dismissed when conflicting with cues from other senses.


1954 ◽  
Vol 30 (4) ◽  
pp. 303-318 ◽  
Author(s):  
K. BOYAN ◽  
E. JALAVISTO

2009 ◽  
Vol 28 (2) ◽  
pp. 179-196 ◽  
Author(s):  
Patricia V. Roehling ◽  
Mark V. Roehling ◽  
Jeffrey D. Vandlen ◽  
Justin Blazek ◽  
William C. Guy

1981 ◽  
Vol 49 (3) ◽  
pp. 851-855
Author(s):  
Bernice N. Ezeilo

This study examined the criteria used by 138 Zambian children in their responses to Furth's non-verbal weight conservation test via posttest interviews. Thirty seven and seven tenths percent of the children responded by weight, 29% by size, 4.4% by size and weight, and 1.4% by size and shape. Others either did not respond at all or gave other nonrelevant responses. Of the conservers, 46% responded by weight while 53% responded by nonweight criteria. The remaining 1% gave nonrelevant responses. All were expected to respond by weight alone. These results raise some doubt about the validity of evidence for weight conservation among African children, based on Furth's non-verbal technique. To improve communication of the weight concept, it is recommended that this test be modified to include a pretraining in size-weight discrimination. A major obstacle to the effective use of the clinical method, by the non-indigenous, for the study of cognitive processes in Third World environment is the problem of verbal communication, so attempts are made to use non-verbal methods. One such attempt was made by Heron and Simonsson (3) who used Furth's non-verbal conservation test to study weight conservation by Zambian children. Furth (2) developed this technique for studying weight conservation of deaf children. It involves essentially three stages. The first requires practice with weights of different sizes. Second, there is practice with obviously equal and obviously unequal balls of plasticine. The third stage is the test. The two practice stages were to ensure that the children had fully understood that same weight was indicated by a horizontal movement of both hands simultaneously and that a judgment of heavier must be communicated by allowing the hand to fall sharply on the table.


Fat Studies ◽  
2013 ◽  
Vol 2 (1) ◽  
pp. 45-55 ◽  
Author(s):  
Amanda Marie Balkhi ◽  
Mike C. Parent ◽  
Mark Mayor

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document