mental time line
Recently Published Documents


TOTAL DOCUMENTS

16
(FIVE YEARS 2)

H-INDEX

9
(FIVE YEARS 0)

2019 ◽  
Vol 42 ◽  
Author(s):  
Danielle DeNigris ◽  
Patricia J. Brooks

Abstract This commentary relates Hoerl & McCormack's dual systems perspective to models of cognitive development emphasizing representational redescription and the role of culturally constructed tools, including language, in providing flexible formats for thinking. We describe developmental processes that enable children to construct a mental time line, situate themselves in time, and overcome the primacy of the here and now.


2018 ◽  
Vol 8 (1) ◽  
Author(s):  
Filomena Anelli ◽  
Gregory Peters-Founshtein ◽  
Yaen Shreibman ◽  
Elior Moreh ◽  
Chiara Forlani ◽  
...  

Cortex ◽  
2016 ◽  
Vol 78 ◽  
pp. 170-173 ◽  
Author(s):  
Luca Rinaldi ◽  
Francesca Locati ◽  
Laura Parolin ◽  
Nicolò F. Bernardi ◽  
Luisa Girelli

2016 ◽  
Vol 30 (3) ◽  
pp. 338-345 ◽  
Author(s):  
Dario Marin ◽  
Marco Pitteri ◽  
Alessandro Della Puppa ◽  
Francesca Meneghello ◽  
Emanuele Biasutti ◽  
...  

2016 ◽  
Vol 2016 ◽  
pp. 1-8 ◽  
Author(s):  
Caterina Ansuini ◽  
Andrea Cavallo ◽  
Lorenzo Pia ◽  
Cristina Becchio

Recent years have seen accumulating evidence for the proposition that people process time by mapping it onto a linear spatial representation and automatically “project” themselves on an imaginedmental time line. Here, we ask whether people can adopt the temporal perspective of another person when travelling through time. To elucidate similarities and differences between time travelling from one’s own perspective or from the perspective of another person, we asked participants to mentally project themselves or someone else (i.e., a coexperimenter) to different time points. Three basic properties of mental time travel were manipulated: temporal location (i.e., where in time the travel originates: past, present, and future), motion direction (either backwards or forwards), and temporal duration (i.e., the distance to travel: one, three, or five years). We found that time travels originating in the present lasted longer in the self- than in the other-perspective. Moreover, for self-perspective, but not for other-perspective, time was differently scaled depending on where in time the travel originated. In contrast, when considering the direction and the duration of time travelling, no dissimilarities between the self- and the other-perspective emerged. These results suggest that self- and other-projection, despite some differences, share important similarities in structure.


PLoS ONE ◽  
2015 ◽  
Vol 10 (7) ◽  
pp. e0130465 ◽  
Author(s):  
Sylvie Droit-Volet ◽  
Jennifer Coull
Keyword(s):  

2015 ◽  
Vol 6 ◽  
Author(s):  
Xianfeng Ding ◽  
Ning Feng ◽  
Xiaorong Cheng ◽  
Huashan Liu ◽  
Zhao Fan
Keyword(s):  

2013 ◽  
Vol 14 (3) ◽  
pp. 231-244 ◽  
Author(s):  
Bettina Rolke ◽  
Susana Ruiz Fernández ◽  
Mareike Schmid ◽  
Matthias Walker ◽  
Martin Lachmair ◽  
...  

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document