scholarly journals Wedge-less oblique supracondylar femoral osteotomy and casting for genu valgum in older children and young adults

2022 ◽  
Vol 25 ◽  
pp. 101730
Author(s):  
Navneet Rustagi ◽  
Altaf Hussain
PEDIATRICS ◽  
1960 ◽  
Vol 26 (2) ◽  
pp. 331-332
Author(s):  
◽  
Lewis L. Coriell ◽  
John H. Dent ◽  
Horace L. Hodes ◽  
C. Henry Kempe ◽  
...  

Immunization with formaldehyde-inactivated vaccine is recommended for all children and young adults. The course recommended earlier was two 1-ml intramuscular injections (1 month apart) followed by a third 1-ml injection (after an interval of approximately 7 months). This is probably adequate for older children and adults. Preliminary evidence indicates that a considerable proportion of infants have a less than optimal or relatively short-lived antibody response when immunized in this fashion. Vaccination of infants should be started at about 1½ to 2 months of age. The Committee recommends that children under 5 years of age be given a series of three 1-mi injections (1 month apart) followed by a fourth injection (up to 8 months later) and then a fifth injection (at about 4 yearsof age). In the present state of knowledge, additional injections seem advisable as indicated in Table I. A recall injection should be given before traveling abroad or before entering an epidemic area. Preparations containing inactivated poliovirus vaccine combined with diphtheria and tetanus toxoids and pertussis bacilli are now available. Such quadruple preparations should be given according to the manufacturer's directions, in general adhering to the schedule presently used for DPT (see Table I). For reasons of safety and to avoid possible loss of antigenic potency, it is considered advisable for physicians not to improvise mixtures of combined diphtheria, pertussis, and tetanus antigens and poliomyelitis vaccine. See table in the PDF file There are few contraindications to poliomyelitis vaccination. It may be performed safely at any time of the year, even when poliomyelitis is prevalent.


2018 ◽  
Author(s):  
Pawel J Matusz ◽  
Rebecca Merkley ◽  
Michelle Faure ◽  
Gaia Scerif

Traditional models developed within cognitive psychology suggest that attention is deployed flexibly and irrespective of differences in expertise with to-be-attended stimuli. However, everyday environments are inherently multisensory and observers differ in familiarity with particular unisensory representations (e.g., number words, in contrast with digits). To test whether the predictions of the traditional models extend to such naturalistic settings, six-year-olds, 11-year-olds and young adults (N=83) searched for predefined numerals amongst a small or large number of distractor digits, while distractor number words, digits or their combination were presented peripherally. Concurrently presented number words and audiovisual stimuli that were compatible with the target digit facilitated young children’s selective attention. In contrast, for older children and young adults number words and audiovisual stimuli that were incompatible with their visual targets resulted in a cost on reaction time. These findings suggest that multisensory and familiarity-based influences interact dynamically as they shape selective attention. Therefore, models of selective attention should include multisensory and familiarity-dependent constraints.


CHEST Journal ◽  
1996 ◽  
Vol 109 (3) ◽  
pp. 673-679 ◽  
Author(s):  
Richard P. Millman ◽  
Christine Acebo ◽  
Cynthia Rosenberg ◽  
Mary A. Carskadon

CHEST Journal ◽  
1996 ◽  
Vol 109 (3) ◽  
pp. 664-672 ◽  
Author(s):  
Christine Acebo ◽  
Richard P. Millman ◽  
Cynthia Rosenberg ◽  
Anita Cavallo ◽  
Mary A. Carskadon

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document