Olfactory detection threshold of ordinary volatile chemicals in mice

2007 ◽  
Vol 58 ◽  
pp. S101
Author(s):  
Yoshika Kurokawa ◽  
Rieko Hojo ◽  
Daisuke Nakajima ◽  
Hidekazu Fujimaki
2014 ◽  
Vol 151 (3) ◽  
pp. 438-442
Author(s):  
Ayotunde James Fasunla ◽  
David Dayo Douglas ◽  
Aderemi Adeleke Adeosun ◽  
Silke Steinbach ◽  
Onyekwere George Benjamin Nwaorgu

1994 ◽  
Vol 8 (5) ◽  
pp. 259-264 ◽  
Author(s):  
Shigeru Furuta ◽  
Kengo Nishimoto ◽  
Masahiko Egawa ◽  
Masaru Ohyama ◽  
Hiroyuki Moriyama

We studied the olfactory functions of organic mercury poisoning (Minamata disease) by using both the smell identification test and the olfactory detection threshold test (with phenyl ethyl alcohol). The subjects were 19 patients with Minamata disease who were treated in Meisuien, Minamata, Kumamoto, Japan and include cases that developed the disease in utero. The mean age was 78.7 ± 14.3 years old. Both smell identification and olfactory detection tests in the majority of patients decreased significantly compared with those of healthy elder subjects. A few cases showed normal olfactory identification and detection functions. The olfactory identification function deceased with advancing age. Correlation existed between detection threshold and background factors such as age and duration after documented Minamata disease was found. No significant relationship was identified between olfactory function and the typical symptoms of Minamata disease.


2008 ◽  
Vol 39 (3) ◽  
pp. 186-191 ◽  
Author(s):  
Rieko HOJO ◽  
Yoshika KUROKAWA ◽  
Shinji TSUKAHARA ◽  
Daisuke NAKAJIMA ◽  
Hidekazu FUJIMAKI

2002 ◽  
Vol 205 (11) ◽  
pp. 1633-1643 ◽  
Author(s):  
Matthias Laska ◽  
Alexandra Seibt

SUMMARY The view that primates are microsmatic animals is based mainly on an interpretation of neuroanatomical features, whereas physiological evidence of a poorly developed sense of smell in this order of mammals is largely lacking. Using a conditioning paradigm, we therefore assessed the olfactory sensitivity of three squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) and of four pigtail macaques (Macaca nemestrina) for a homologous series of aliphatic alcohols (ethanol to 1-octanol) and isomeric forms of some of these substances. In the majority of cases, the animals of both species significantly discriminated concentrations below 1 part per million from the odourless solvent, and with 1-hexanol individual monkeys even demonstrated thresholds below 10 parts per billion. The results showed (i) that both primate species have a well-developed olfactory sensitivity for aliphatic alcohols, which for the majority of substances matches or even is better than that of species such as the rat, (ii) that both species generally show very similar olfactory detection thresholds for aliphatic alcohols, and (iii) that a significant negative correlation between perceptibility in terms of olfactory detection threshold and carbon chain length of both the aliphatic 1-and 2-alcohols exists in both species. These findings support the idea that across-species comparisons of neuroanatomical features are a poor predictor of olfactory performance and that general labels such as `microsmat' or`macrosmat', which are usually based on allometric comparisons of olfactory brain structures, are inadequate to describe the olfactory capabilities of a species. Further, our findings suggest that olfaction may play an important and hitherto underestimated role in the regulation of behaviour in the species tested.


2019 ◽  
Vol 9 (9) ◽  
pp. 986-992 ◽  
Author(s):  
Richard L. Doty ◽  
Crystal Wylie ◽  
Mark Potter ◽  
Rachel Beston ◽  
Brooke Cope ◽  
...  

2008 ◽  
Vol 33 (5) ◽  
pp. 461-467 ◽  
Author(s):  
J. Albrecht ◽  
A. Anzinger ◽  
R. Kopietz ◽  
V. Schopf ◽  
A. M. Kleemann ◽  
...  

2020 ◽  
Author(s):  
Coralie Mignot ◽  
Philipp Nahrath ◽  
Charlotte Sinding ◽  
Thomas Hummel

Abstract Olfactory habituation corresponds to a decreased behavioral or perceptual response to an odor after a prolonged exposure to this odor. Our aim was to investigate whether long-term olfactory habituation and its recovery is similar in young (<35 years old) and older adults (>50). Fifty seven participants were recruited for a five week longitudinal study. They were exposed to one of two odors (manzanate, irone alpha) for two weeks at home. Olfactory detection thresholds for both odors were measured before and after exposure. Results showed that the two age groups behaved similarly. The long-term exposure to an odor led to a temporary increase of its detection threshold (lower sensitivity to the odor). Irone alpha thresholds were more sensitive to the duration of exposure with the odor than manzanate thresholds. One week after termination of exposure, participants fully recovered and even became more sensitive to both odors. No cross-habituation was found between the two odors. Our findings highlight that long-term habituation is specific to the odor exposed, behaves the same in young and older adults and is fully reversible in both age groups after one week.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document