Attentional deficit in the siblings of schizophrenics

1979 ◽  
Vol 9 (3) ◽  
pp. 465-467 ◽  
Author(s):  
Rodger L. Wood ◽  
Mark cook

SynopsisA study of attentional deficit in the relatives of schizophrenics and in a matched control group is reported, in which the relatives are found to have poorer scores on a vigilance test and on a choice reaction time test. It is concluded that this deficit points to one of the primary symptoms of schizophrenia, and suggestions for further research are made

1980 ◽  
Vol 8 (6) ◽  
pp. 606-611 ◽  
Author(s):  
Steven P. Mewaldt ◽  
Cheryl L. Connelly ◽  
J. Richard Simon

1975 ◽  
Vol 7 (5) ◽  
pp. 441-446 ◽  
Author(s):  
Robert T. Wilkinson ◽  
David Houghton

1996 ◽  
Vol 82 (3_suppl) ◽  
pp. 1307-1315 ◽  
Author(s):  
Shulan Hsieh ◽  
Wen-Juh Hwang ◽  
Jing-Jane Tsai ◽  
Chin-Yi Tsai

Orienting attention to visual stimuli was studied in 13 patients with Parkinson's disease whose responses were compared to those of a matched control group using a cued reaction-time task which measured cost and benefit effects of orienting of attention. Both groups were screened to exclude dementia, psychiatric disease, and other neurological abnormalities. Although Parkinson patients showed over-all slow mean reaction time, responses showed a pattern of cost and benefit effects similar to that of the control group. The results suggested that Parkinson patients are not impaired on visuospatial orienting of attention on this task.


1980 ◽  
Vol 10 (2) ◽  
pp. 381-382 ◽  
Author(s):  
Keith Millar ◽  
Cristine Walder

SYNOPSISRe-analysis is made of the data from a recent study purporting toshow an attentional deficit in the siblings of schizophrenics when compared with a matched control group. The re-analysis fails to support the previously reported levels of statistical reliability and may question the inferences to be drawn from the study.


Author(s):  
Krzysztof Przednowek ◽  
Maciej Śliż ◽  
Justyna Lenik ◽  
Bartosz Dziadek ◽  
Stanisław Cieszkowski ◽  
...  

The main purpose of the paper was to evaluate selected psychomotor abilities of handball players depending on the competition class (league), position on the court, training seniority and the dominant hand. The study covered a group of 40 handball players (age: 24.02 ± 3.99), while 50 non-training men (age: 22.90 ± 1.13) formed the control group. Studies were performed using Test2Drive computer tests. The following four tests were used for measuring psychomotor fitness: simple reaction time test, choice reaction time test, hand-eye coordination test and spatial anticipation test. An analysis revealed that handball players had better reaction times and movement times than the control group. The league, position on the court, training seniority and the dominant upper limb were analysed for their impact on the reaction time and movement time in handball players. An analysis of psychomotor abilities of handball players with regard to the league revealed that in the majority of tests the Superliga players had a shorter reaction time than players in lower leagues.


2019 ◽  
Vol 34 (4) ◽  
pp. 833-841
Author(s):  
Taras I. Usichenko ◽  
Danika Städing ◽  
Michael Boesche ◽  
Henriette Janner ◽  
Thomas Hesse ◽  
...  

1993 ◽  
Vol 21 (2) ◽  
pp. 180-184 ◽  
Author(s):  
J. Noble ◽  
J. G. Jones ◽  
E. J. Davis

The effect of hypoxaemia (mean SpO2 78%) on cognitive function was measured in two groups of twelve normal subjects. A series of psychometric tests was administered to each subject in the same sequence and consisted of the Reitan trail-making test, a digit symbol substitution test, a visuospatial orientation test and the simple unprepared reaction-time test. Psychomotor performance was assessed in a double-blind manner while the subjects were breathing first air and then either air or a hypoxic mixture. While there was improvement in time for the trail-making test during a repeat study breathing air, there was significant deterioration of time to completion of the test in conditions of hypoxia. A significant learning effect in the orientation test was seen in the control group but this did not occur in hypoxic subjects. Hypoxaemia was shown to cause a significant impairment of simple unprepared reaction time compared with controls. All the changes in cognitive function were small and there were no subjective differences in the air or hypoxic groups. The usefulness of the Reitan trail-making and the simple unprepared reaction-time test in the assessment of psychomotor performance deficit under conditions of hypoxaemia has been demonstrated by this study in normal subjects. It was concluded that a mean oxygen saturation of 78% caused only minor changes in cognitive function in normal subjects.


2001 ◽  
Vol 24 (2) ◽  
pp. 91-94 ◽  
Author(s):  
Jo??lle Micallef-Roll ◽  
Philippe Rihet ◽  
Thierry Hasbroucq ◽  
Camille Possama?? ◽  
Olivier Blin

2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
James R.C. Davis ◽  
Silvin P. Knight ◽  
Orna A. Donoghue ◽  
Belinda Hernández ◽  
Rose Anne Kenny ◽  
...  

Gait speed is a measure of general fitness. Changing from usual (UGS) to maximum (MGS) gait speed requires a general effort across many body systems. The difference, MGS − UGS, is defined as gait speed reserve (GSR). In the present study, using 3925 participants aged 50+ from Wave 3 of The Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing (TILDA), we used a gradient boosted trees-based stepwise feature selection pipeline for the discovery of clinically relevant predictors of GSR, UGS, and MGS using a shortlist of 88 features across 5 categories (socio-demographics/anthropometrics/medical history; cardiovascular system; physical strength; sensory; and cognitive/psychological). The TreeSHAP explainable machine learning package was used to analyse the input-output relationships of the three models. The mean R2adj (SD) from 5-fold cross validation on training data and the R2adj score on test data for the models are: 0.38 (0.04) and 0.41 for UGS; 0.45 (0.04) and 0.46 for MGS; and 0.19 (0.02) and 0.21 for GSR. Features selected for the UGS model were: age, chair stands time, body mass index, grip strength, number of medications, resting state pulse interval, mean motor reaction time in the choice reaction time test, height, depression score, sit-to-stand difference in diastolic blood pressure, and left visual acuity. The features selected for the MGS model were: age, grip strength, repeated chair stands time, body mass index, education, mean motor reaction time in the choice reaction time test, number of medications, height, the standard deviation of the mean reaction time in the sustained attention to response task, mean heart rate at resting state, fear of falling, MOCA errors, orthostatic intolerance during active stand, smoking status, total heart beat power during paced breathing, the root mean square of successive differences between heartbeats during paced breathing, and visual acuity. Finally, the features chosen for the GSR model were: mean motor reaction time in the choice reaction time test, grip strength, education, chair stands time, MOCA errors, accuracy proportion in the sound induced flash illusion (two beeps and one flash with stimulus-onset asynchrony of +150 ms), fear of falling, height, age, sex, orthostatic intolerance, MMSE errors, and number of cardiovascular conditions. MGS and UGS were more explainable than GSR. All three models contain features from all five categories. There were common features to all three models (age, grip strength, chair stands time, mean motor reaction time in the choice reaction time test, and height), but also some features unique to each of them. Overall, findings on all three models were clinically plausible and support a network physiology approach to the understanding of predictors of performance-based tasks. By employing an explainable machine learning technique, our observations may help clinicians gain new insights into the multisystem predictors of gait speed and gait speed reserve in older adults.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document