scholarly journals Correction to: Age-related changes to environmental exposure: variation in the frequency that young children place hands and objects in their mouths

2019 ◽  
Vol 30 (4) ◽  
pp. 769-772 ◽  
Author(s):  
Laura H. Kwong ◽  
Ayse Ercumen ◽  
Amy J. Pickering ◽  
Leanne Unicomb ◽  
Jennifer Davis ◽  
...  
2019 ◽  
Vol 30 (1) ◽  
pp. 205-216 ◽  
Author(s):  
Laura H. Kwong ◽  
Ayse Ercumen ◽  
Amy J. Pickering ◽  
Leanne Unicomb ◽  
Jennifer Davis ◽  
...  

2003 ◽  
Vol 114 (4) ◽  
pp. 2395-2395
Author(s):  
Mathew Parson ◽  
Amanda Lloyd ◽  
Kelly Stoddard ◽  
Shawn L. Nissen

1992 ◽  
Vol 335 (1273) ◽  
pp. 95-103 ◽  

Young children do not form representations of newly encountered faces as efficiently as do adults. A first step in explaining this difference, like any age-related change, is locating its source. A major source of the improvement is acquisition of knowledge of faces per se , as opposed to age-related changes in general pattern encoding or memorial skills. Two consequences of expertise at individualizing members of classes that share a basic configuration are known: a large inversion effect and a caricature advantage. It is possible that both of these effects reflect increased reliance, with expertise, on configuration distinguishing features. Several phenomena that indicate that inversion interferes with the encoding of configural aspects of faces are reviewed. Finally, developmental data are presented that confirm the suspicion that there are at least two distinct sources of the vulnerability of face encoding to inversion, perhaps reflecting two distinct senses of ‘configural encoding’ of faces, only one of which is implicated in adult expertise at face encoding.


2021 ◽  
pp. 174569162096407
Author(s):  
Melanie Killen ◽  
Audun Dahl

Moral reasoning is an essential part of how humans develop and a fundamental aspect of how human societies change over time. On a developmental timescale, reasoning about interpersonal disagreements and dilemmas spurs age-related changes in moral judgments from childhood to adulthood. When asked to distribute resources among others, even young children strive to balance competing concerns with equality, merit, and need. Over the course of development, reasoning and judgments about resource distribution and other moral issues become increasingly sophisticated. From childhood to adulthood, individuals not only evaluate acts as right or wrong but also take the extra steps to rectify inequalities, protest unfair norms, and resist stereotypic expectations about others. The development of moral reasoning also enables change on a societal timescale. Across centuries and communities, ordinary individuals have called for societal change based on moral concerns with welfare, rights, fairness, and justice. Individuals have effectively employed reasoning to identify and challenge injustices. In this article, we synthesize recent insights from developmental science about the roles of moral reasoning in developmental and societal change. In the concluding section, we turn to questions for future research on moral reasoning and change.


2014 ◽  
Vol 9 ◽  
pp. 93-105 ◽  
Author(s):  
Jennie K. Grammer ◽  
Melisa Carrasco ◽  
William J. Gehring ◽  
Frederick J. Morrison

1995 ◽  
Vol 18 (4) ◽  
pp. 701-711 ◽  
Author(s):  
Anat Scher ◽  
Emanuel Tirosh ◽  
Michael Jaffe ◽  
Lisa Rubin ◽  
Avi Sadeh ◽  
...  

The sleep habits of 661 Israeli children between the ages of 4 months and 4 years were described by their mothers. Twenty-eight per cent reported that their children woke up at least once a week. In the group of regular wakers, the mean number of interrupted nights per week was 4.7, and the mean number of awakenings per night was 2.0. Significant age-related changes in sleep patterns were indicated. The results of this study suggest that sleep and settling patterns in different sociocultural groups are quite similar. These data indicate the existence of an inherent pattern in the maturation of sleep behaviour in the developing child.


1993 ◽  
Vol 16 (1) ◽  
pp. 93-111 ◽  
Author(s):  
S. J. Murgatroyd ◽  
E. J. Robinson

The aim of this research was to examine contradictions between three published accounts of age-related changes in children's judgements of the emotion experienced by a wrongdoer: accounts by Barden, Zelco, Duncan, and Masters (1980), Nunner-Winkler and Sodian (1988), and Harter and Whitesell (1989). We report three studies involving children aged between 4 and 10 years who watched dolls enacting scenes involving a wrongdoer and then judged how that doll felt, and one involving adults who made an emotion attribution for story characters. Contrary to Harter and Whitesell, many children did judge a wrongdoer to feel happy, but contrary to Barden et al. and to Nunner-Winkler and Sodian, the incidence of happy judgements did not decline with age, and they remained even among adults. Contrary to Barden et al., "happy" judgements were no less common when children believed their teacher was going to see their answers. Contrary to Harter and Whitesell and to Barden et al., judgements of "sad" rather than "scared" were given by some young children. The developmental picture remains unclear.


2001 ◽  
Vol 6 (4) ◽  
pp. 254-263 ◽  
Author(s):  
G.C.M. van Baal ◽  
C.E.M. van Beijsterveldt ◽  
P.C.M. Molenaar ◽  
D.I. Boomsma ◽  
E.J.C. de Geus

Changes in genetic and environmental influences on electroencephalographic (EEG) and event-related potential (ERP) indices of neural development were studied in two large cohorts of young (N = 418) and adolescent (N = 426) twins. Individual differences in these indices were largely influenced by genetic factors, and throughout development, the stable part of the variance was mainly genetic. Both EEG power (which describes the amount of variability in brain electrical potentials that can be attributed to different frequencies) and long-distance EEG coherence (which is the squared cross-correlation between two EEG signals at different scalp locations and can be regarded as an index for cortico-cortical connectivity) were highly heritable. ERP-P300 latencies and amplitudes were low to moderately heritable. Clear differences between young children and adolescents could be observed in the heritabilities of EEG and ERP indices. The heritabilities of EEG power and EEG coherence were higher in adolescents than in children, whereas the heritabilities of P300 latencies were lower. Both cohorts (young children and adolescents) were measured twice: The children were tested when they were 5 and again at 7 years, the adolescents when they were 16 and again at 18 years. Therefore, within these age ranges a more detailed analysis of age-related changes in heritabilities and in the emergence of new genetic influences could be studied. The heritabilities of EEG powers and P300 amplitudes and latencies did not change much from age 5 to age 7 and from age 16 to 18 years. The heritabilities of a substantial number of connections within the cortex, however, as indexed by EEG coherence, changed significantly from age 5 to age 7, though not from age 16 to 18. The only changes in the heritabilities in adolescents were connections within the prefrontal cortex, which is in agreement with theories of adolescent development. These age-related changes in the heritabilities may reflect a larger impact of maturation on cortico-cortical connectivity in childhood than in adolescence. Evidence was found for qualitative changes in brain electrophysiology in young children: New genetic factors emerged at age 7 for posterior EEG coherences and for P300 latency at some scalp locations. This supports theories of qualitative stage transitions in this age range, as previously suggested using behavioral and EEG data.


2005 ◽  
Vol 28 (2) ◽  
pp. 617-644 ◽  
Author(s):  
Donaya Hongwanishkul ◽  
Keith R. Happaney ◽  
Wendy S. C. Lee ◽  
Philip David Zelazo

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document