A Comparison of Sample Sizes for the Analysis of Means and the Analysis of Variance

1983 ◽  
Vol 15 (1) ◽  
pp. 33-39 ◽  
Author(s):  
Peter R. Nelson
1968 ◽  
Vol 27 (2) ◽  
pp. 363-367 ◽  
Author(s):  
John E. Overall ◽  
Sudhir N. Dalal

Simple empirical formulae are presented for estimating appropriate sample size for simple randomized analysis of variance designs involving 2, 3, 4 or 5 treatments. In order to use these formulae one must specify the magnitude of a meaningful treatment difference and must have an estimate of the error variance. Sample size estimates derived from the simple formulae have been found to differ from values obtained using constant power curves by no more than one sampling unit on the low side and no more than two sampling units on the high side.


2017 ◽  
Author(s):  
Robert S. Chavez ◽  
Dylan D. Wagner

AbstractWhole-brain analysis of variance (ANOVA) is a common analytic approach in cognitive neuroscience. Researchers are often interested in exploring whether brain activity reflects to the interaction of two factors. Disordinal interactions — where there is a reversal of the effect of one independent variable at a level of a second independent variable — are common in the literature. It is well established in power-analyses of factorial ANOVAs that certain patterns of interactions, such as disordinal (e.g., cross-over interactions) require less power than others to detect. This fact, combined with the perils of mass univariate testing suggests that testing for interactions in whole-brain ANOVAs, may be biased towards the detection of disordinal interactions. Here, we report on a series of simulated analysis --including whole-brain fMRI data using realistic multi-source noise parameters-- that demonstrate a bias towards the detection of disordinal interactions in mass-univariate contexts. Moreover, results of these simulations indicated that spurious disordinal interactions are found at common thresholds and cluster sizes at the group level. Moreover, simulations based on implanting true ordinal interaction effects can nevertheless appear like crossover effects at realistic levels of signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) when performing mass univariate testing at the whole-brain level, potentially leading to erroneous conclusions when interpreted as is. Simulations of varying sample sizes and SNR levels show that this bias is driven primarily by SNR and larger sample sizes do little to ameliorate this issue. Together, the results of these simulations argue for caution when searching for ordinal interactions in whole-brain ANOVA.


Author(s):  
Saad T. Bakir

Analysis of Means by Ranks is a nonparametric statistical test procedure that was developed in Bakir (1989) but has rarely been applied in practice. This paper modifies and applies Analysis of Means by Ranks to a case study data involving the comparison of three contract proposals. For comparison purposes, we analyze the same data using the well-known Analysis of Variance, Analysis of Means, and the Kruskal-Wallis test. Analysis of Variance and Analysis of Means are two parametric (assume data to be samples from normal populations) test procedures whereas Kruskal-Wallis and Analysis of Means by Ranks are two nonparametric (or distribution-free) procedures. This paper shows that the parametric tests fail to detect a significant difference among three contract proposals, while the nonparametric tests do.  The conclusions of the parametric tests are in doubt because a descriptive statistics analysis indicates that the required normality assumption is in doubt; the nonparametric conclusions are more trustful because the normality assumption is not required by nonparametric procedures.


1970 ◽  
Vol 2 (3) ◽  
pp. 156-164 ◽  
Author(s):  
T. L. Bratcher ◽  
M. A. Moran ◽  
W. J. Zimmer

1980 ◽  
Vol 12 (2) ◽  
pp. 106-113 ◽  
Author(s):  
E. G. Schilling ◽  
G. Schlotzer ◽  
H. E. Schultz ◽  
J. H. Sheesley

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document