Semantic alignment across whole-number arithmetic and rational numbers: evidence from a Russian perspective

2017 ◽  
Vol 24 (2) ◽  
pp. 198-220 ◽  
Author(s):  
Yulia A. Tyumeneva ◽  
Galina Larina ◽  
Ekaterina Alexandrova ◽  
Melissa DeWolf ◽  
Miriam Bassok ◽  
...  
1988 ◽  
Vol 19 (2) ◽  
pp. 175-180
Author(s):  
Robert P. Hunting ◽  
Christopher F. Sharpley

Much school mathematics is devoted to teaching concepts and procedures based on those units that form the core of whole number arithmetic (ones, tens, hundreds, etc.). But other topics such as fractions and decimals demand a new and extended understanding of units and their relationships. Behr, Wachsmuth, Post, and Lesh (1984) and Streefland (1984) have noted how children's whole number ideas interfere with their efforts to learn fractions. Hunting (1986) suggested that a reason children seem to have difficulty learning stable and appropriate meanings for fractions is that instruction on fractions, if delayed too long, allows whole number knowledge to become the predominant scheme to which fraction language and symbolism is then related.


Author(s):  
Ferdinando Arzarello ◽  
Nadia Azrou ◽  
Maria G. Bartolini Bussi ◽  
Sarah Inés González de Lora Sued ◽  
Xu Hua Sun ◽  
...  

2002 ◽  
Vol 11 (5) ◽  
pp. 181-185 ◽  
Author(s):  
Mark H. Ashcraft

Highly math-anxious individuals are characterized by a strong tendency to avoid math, which ultimately undercuts their math competence and forecloses important career paths. But timed, on-line tests reveal math-anxiety effects on whole-number arithmetic problems (e.g., 46 + 27), whereas achievement tests show no competence differences. Math anxiety disrupts cognitive processing by compromising ongoing activity in working memory. Although the causes of math anxiety are undetermined, some teaching styles are implicated as risk factors. We need research on the origins of math anxiety and on its “signature” in brain activity, to examine both its emotional and its cognitive components.


2017 ◽  
Vol 2 (3) ◽  
pp. 170-189 ◽  
Author(s):  
Andreas Obersteiner ◽  
Veronika Hofreiter

Number sense requires, at least, an ability to assess magnitude information represented by number symbols. Most educated adults are able to assess magnitude information of rational numbers fairly quickly, including whole numbers and fractions. It is to date unclear whether educated adults without training are able to assess magnitudes of irrational numbers, such as the cube root of 41. In a computerized experiment, we asked mathematically skilled adults to repeatedly choose the larger of two irrational numbers as quickly as possible. Participants were highly accurate on problems in which reasoning about the exact or approximate value of the irrational numbers’ whole number components (e.g., 3 and 41 in the cube root of 41) yielded the correct response. However, they performed at random chance level when these strategies were invalid and the problem required reasoning about the irrational number magnitudes as a whole. Response times suggested that participants hardly even tried to assess magnitudes of the irrational numbers as a whole, and if they did, were largely unsuccessful. We conclude that even mathematically skilled adults struggle with quickly assessing magnitudes of irrational numbers in their symbolic notation. Without practice, number sense seems to be restricted to rational numbers.


1985 ◽  
Vol 32 (7) ◽  
pp. 36-40
Author(s):  
Cornelia C. Tierney

In elementary school, great importance is placed on memorization of number facts. In teaching fifth through eighth graders, 1 have assumed that most of my students had made a concerted effort to memorize facts in earlier grades. I have observed. however, that children who have had a similar amount of practice have a great range of recall. A few students complete tests of 100 multiplication or division facts perfectly in less than three minutes, whereas others are made miserable by the whole process. They skip many problems, look around the room to compare their progress with that of other students, and finally give up with few correct answers. Although those who have memorized the facts do better than others at whole-number arithmetic, they do not always do well in work with fractions.


1984 ◽  
Vol 31 (6) ◽  
pp. 10-12
Author(s):  
Dora Helen B. Skypek

The characteristics of rational numbers must be considered in a variety of interpretations and coding schemes. It is this variety that, if not sorted and carefully developed in appropriate contexts, results in confusion in teaching and learning about rational numbers. Although a discussion of interpretations necessarily involves the use of coding conventions, the two will be treated separately as special characteristics of the rational numbers. Another important charac-teristic. which is difficult to separate from interpretations and coding conventions. is this: unlike a whole number (or an integer), a rational number has an unlimited number of “behavioral clones.” These clones have different names, but they behave in exactly the same way. Still other Still other characteristics to be considered are the density and order of the rational number.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document