Effects of Stimulus Probability on Reaction Time in a Number-Naming Task

1964 ◽  
Vol 16 (1) ◽  
pp. 47-55 ◽  
Author(s):  
George C. Stone ◽  
Enoch Callaway
1975 ◽  
Vol 41 (2) ◽  
pp. 607-613 ◽  
Author(s):  
A. R. Blackman

In a test of Sternberg's (1969) additive-factor method of reaction time (RT) analysis, stimulus quality (intact, degraded), relative stimulus frequency (70:15:15), and S-R compatibility (naming, naming-plus-one), each had significant effects on RT in a number-naming task. Additivity of the means, variances, and third cumulants of RT for the quality and compatibility factors showed the RT components attributable to the two stages influenced by these two variables to be stochastically independent. Relative stimulus frequency interacted with both quality and compatibility in determining RT. It was concluded that the effect of stimulus quality arose at the stimulus encoding stage, and the effect of compatibility at the S-R translation stage, while the duration of both stages was influenced by relative stimulus frequency.


1989 ◽  
Vol 69 (3-2) ◽  
pp. 1243-1250
Author(s):  
Janet D. Larsen ◽  
Cheryl K. Yatsko ◽  
Bonnie McCulley ◽  
Thomas Fritsch

In three studies, no evidence of nonconscious perception was found, although general procedures used in previous studies reporting the effect were followed. Presence/absence thresholds (Exps. 1 and 2) or recognition threshold (Exp. 3) were established for each subject. There was no difference in the effects of related and unrelated primes on voice reaction time during a word naming task. These findings raise questions about the robustness of nonconscious priming effects.


Cortex ◽  
1970 ◽  
Vol 6 (1) ◽  
pp. 97-101
Author(s):  
Edoardo Bisiach ◽  
Giuseppe Frangini

2018 ◽  
Author(s):  
Tali Leibovich-Raveh ◽  
Daniel J. Lewis ◽  
Daniel Ansari

contains a matlab code for calculating individual subitizing range (ISR), instructions for using the code, and a ready to run experiment - number naming task that is suitable for both children and adults (requires OpenSesame 3.07).


2020 ◽  
pp. 136700692095187
Author(s):  
Carla E Contreras-Saavedra ◽  
Iring Koch ◽  
Stefanie Schuch ◽  
Andrea M Philipp

Aims and objectives/purpose/research questions: Language-switch costs, which denote worse performance in language-switch than in language-repetition trials, appear to be a robust finding in bilingual language switching. The aim of the present study was to examine the intraindividual reliability of language-switch costs by means of a number-naming task with German-English bilinguals. Design/methodology/approach: In a cued language-switching paradigm, participants ( n = 36) switched between German and English. They performed a number-naming task in three different conditions: one-digit numbers; two-digit numbers with decade 10; and two-digit numbers with decade 20. Data and analysis: We examined the experimental effects with an analysis of variance (reaction time and error rate as the dependent variables), using trial language, language sequence and number condition as independent variables. In addition, we calculated the split-half reliability of language-switch costs (across all conditions) as well as the correlations of language-switch costs between the different conditions. Findings/conclusions: While significant language-switch costs emerged in all three number conditions, our results demonstrate a medium-sized correlation between the three experimental conditions. The split-half reliability shows a moderate to strong correlation between the odd- and even-numbered trials in the experiment. Originality: On the one hand, the present study extends the observation of language-switch costs from one-digit number naming to the more complex naming of two-digit numbers. On the other hand, and theoretically even more important, we explored the reliability of language-switch costs in a bilingual number-naming task by calculating both split-half reliability and correlations between different number conditions. Significance/implications: The results indicated that while language-switch costs are a robust experimental effect on the group level, they appear to be less well suited for correlational approaches. This also suggests that caution should be exerted when language-switch costs are used to diagnose the ability of an individual to perform language control.


2020 ◽  
Vol 73 (9) ◽  
pp. 1481-1494
Author(s):  
Carla E Contreras-Saavedra ◽  
Klaus Willmes ◽  
Iring Koch ◽  
Stefanie Schuch ◽  
Elena Benini ◽  
...  

The aim of this study was to examine language switching in a two-digit number naming task. In contrast to single digits, two-digit numbers have a composition rule (i.e., morphological configuration) that may differ between languages. For example, the Arabic number 21 is read with an inverted composition rule in German (unit before decade) and a non-inverted composition rule in English (decade before unit). In the present experiment, one group of German native speakers and one group of Spanish native speakers had to name two-digit numbers in German, English, or Spanish. The results demonstrate a language-switch cost, revealing better performance in language repetition than in language-switch trials. This switch cost was further modulated by repeating or switching the composition rule, since the language repetition benefit (i.e., the switch cost) was reduced in trials with composition-rule switches compared with trials with composition-rule repetitions. This finding indicates that the language in which the number word has to be produced and its composition rule are not switched independently but rather may be integrated into one language schema.


Nature ◽  
1967 ◽  
Vol 215 (5103) ◽  
pp. 895-896 ◽  
Author(s):  
I. E. GORDON

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document