scholarly journals The Pickup of Nonspecifying Variables Does Not Entail Indirect Perception

2004 ◽  
Vol 16 (3) ◽  
pp. 237-253 ◽  
Author(s):  
Rob Withagen
Keyword(s):  
2021 ◽  
Vol 02 (05) ◽  
pp. 72-77
Author(s):  
Ganiev A.G. ◽  

The article discusses the factors that improve people’s intellectual abilities. To do this, it provides information about the physiology of the human brain and the mechanism by which the information it perceives is stored in memory. To develop students' 'creative thinking' skills, it is necessary to develop their 'imagination' skills. The imagination perceives information from the mind. “Indirect perception” is important for imagining physical processes. Data visualization is important for understanding processes. To do this, use the "Mind Map", which is an effective way to perform such a task. The article describes the opportunities for students to develop "creative thinking" skills in the teaching of "Aggregate states of matter." For the first time, "Aggregate Cases" are being published. Not only it stores a lot of information about physical processes, but it also helps students develop “full thinking” skills by activating the cerebral hemispheres.


1981 ◽  
Vol 31 (125) ◽  
pp. 330 ◽  
Author(s):  
E. J. Lowe

2002 ◽  
Vol 130 (5-6) ◽  
pp. 183-188
Author(s):  
Natasa Ivanovic ◽  
Marina Svetel ◽  
Dusko Kozic ◽  
Robert Semnic ◽  
Vladimir Kostic

Symptomatic dystonia can be the result of various metabolic, degenerative diseases, the consumption of certain medications or exposure to toxic agents. However, only symptomatic dystonia with focal structural lesion provides a significant "window" for, at least indirect, perception of aetiopa-thogenesis and pathomorphological substratum of idiopathic dystonia. Our study included 57 patients with symptomatic dystonia, which as a base had focal or multifocal lesions, of whom 7 patients had generalized dystonia, 18 hemidystonia, 6 segmental dystonia, 7 torticollis, 6 blepharospasm, 7 hand dystonia, 3 spasmodic dysphonia, and 3 had oromandibular dystonia. Stroke was highly statistically the most frequent cause of structural lesions (33/57 or 58%). Relevant pathomorphological changes were present in 50/57 (88%) patients, of whom 25 (50%) had lesion in the lenticular nucleus (including individual damage of the putamen and globus pallidus), 12/50 (24%) had damage of the thalamus and 6/50 (12%) had damage of the brainstem. Generalized dystonia was most frequently associated with bilateral lesion of the putamen, hemidystonia with lesion of contralateral putamen, torticollis with damage of the caudate nucleus, hand dystonia with lesion of the thalamus and blepharospasm with lesion of the upper brainstem.


Author(s):  
Osamu Hieda

Kumam is a Western Nilotic language that is spoken in central Uganda. This chapter focuses on the formation of a double downstep high tone, the function of middle sentences, and evidentiality in complementation. Kumam is a tone language with a low and a high toneme, exhibiting a double downstep high tone as a feature. Aspect (imperfective vs. perfective) is marked obligatorily with a suprasegmental morpheme, while tense is not marked in verbal complexes. Tense is expressed lexically. Kumam has no passivization, but middle sentences function as a passive equivalent instead. Kumam has two types of complementation, “paratactic” and “hypotactic”, that are different syntactically and semantically. For instance, when perception verbs are followed by a “paratactic” clause, they express direct perception. When they are followed by a “hypotactic” clause, they express indirect perception. There is the relationship between the complement types and evidentiality.


2002 ◽  
Vol 25 (1) ◽  
pp. 112-113 ◽  
Author(s):  
Jason S. McCarley ◽  
Gregory J. DiGirolamo

Norman's aim to reconcile two longstanding and seemingly opposed philosophies of perception, the constructivist and the ecological, by casting them as approaches to complementary subsystems within the visual brain is laudable. Unfortunately, Norman overreaches in attempting to equate direct perception with dorsal/unconscious visual processing and indirect perception with ventral/conscious visual processing. Even a cursory review suggests that the functional and neural segregation of direct and indirect perception is not as clear as the target article would suggest.


Philosophy ◽  
2004 ◽  
Vol 79 (3) ◽  
pp. 393-402 ◽  
Author(s):  
Scott Campbell

Frank Jackson in Perception uses the ‘in virtue of’ relation to ground the distinction between direct and indirect perception. He argues that it follows that our perception of physical objects is mediated by perceiving their facing surfaces, and so is indirect. I argue that this is false. Seeing a part of an object is in itself a seeing of the object; there is no indirectness involved. Hence, the ‘in virtue of’ relation is an inadequate basis for the direct-indirect distinction. I also argue that claims that we don't, ‘strictly speaking’, see objects, are also false.


1980 ◽  
Vol 28 (4) ◽  
pp. 306-314 ◽  
Author(s):  
Joel Norman
Keyword(s):  

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document