Paired-Associate Transfer as a Function of Ability Level in the A-B, C-A and the A-B, B-C Paradigms

1968 ◽  
Vol 22 (1) ◽  
pp. 203-210
Author(s):  
Richard M. Schulman

In 2 experiments, the A-B, B-C and the A-B, C-A paradigms were compared with the A-B, C-D control using 2 levels of ability. For each experiment a different although equivalent sample of 120 Armor enlisted personnel was used (6 groups of 20 each). Both lists of Exp. I and List I of Exp. II utilized a modification of the anticipation procedure in which the stimuli were not pronounced. List II of Exp. II additionally used a multiple-choice procedure to equate response availability between groups. Original list learning unequivocally demonstrated the superiority of the high ability groups. List II comparisons showed clearly for the A-B, B-C paradigm that the high ability groups were faster but were somewhat equivocal for the A-B, C-A paradigm. The similar pattern of A-B, C-A transfer in both experiments was interpreted as suggesting the possibility of a source of facilitation operating on the perceptual level in Exp. II.

1971 ◽  
Vol 33 (2) ◽  
pp. 527-529 ◽  
Author(s):  
Albert V. Carron

The interactive effects of ability level and a 2-yr. layoff upon forgetting of a balance skill were examined. In an earlier study, Carron and Marteniuk (1970) tested 150 male Ss on the stabilometer, ranked them according to their initial ability level and selected 3 groups ( N = 20 in each) which consisted of the best performers (high-ability group), the worst performers (low-ability group) and the performers scoring closest to the median (average-ability group). No differences were evident following a 1- or a 7-day layoff but a significant interaction (resulting from a reminiscence effect for the high-ability group combined with forgetting for the average- and low-ability groups) was present following a 14-day layoff. Ss from the three ability groups were retested (high ability, N = 9; average ability, N = 8; and low ability, N = 13). While all groups showed forgetting and the high-ability Ss had a considerably smaller percentage of forgetting (23.8% for the high-ability versus 41.5% and 46.0% for the average- and low-ability groups), the interaction between ability groups and forgetting just failed to reach significance.


1995 ◽  
Vol 7 (2) ◽  
pp. 144-152 ◽  
Author(s):  
Arthur P. Shimamura ◽  
Paul J. Jurica ◽  
Jennifer A. Mangels ◽  
Felick B. Gershberg ◽  
Robert T. Knight

Patients with frontal lobe lesions were adminstered tests of paired-associate learning in which cue and response words are manipulated to increase interference across two study lists. In one test of paired-associate learning (AB-AC test), cue words used in one list are repeated in a second list but are associated with different response words (e.g., LION-HUNTER, LION-CIRCUS). In another test (AB-ABr test), words used in one list are repeated in a second list but are rearranged to form new pairs. Compared to control subjects, patients with frontal lobe lesions exhibited disproportionate impairment of second-list learning as a result of interference effects. In particular, patients exhibited the poorest performance during the initial trial of the second list, a trial in which interference effects from the first list would be most apparent. These findings suggest that the on-line control of irrelevant or competing memory associations is disrupted following frontal lobe lesions. This disruption may be indicative of an impaired gating or filtering mechanism that affects not only memory function but other cognitive function as well.


2020 ◽  
Vol 1 (1) ◽  
pp. 21-26
Author(s):  
Abubakar Yahya Ibrahim

Purpose of the study: The purpose of this study was to determine the effect of student ability levels on the average value (x̄) of academic achievement of students in geography and the influence of method interactions and abilities on the average achievement score (x̄) of students in geography Methodology: The research design for the study was Quasi-experimental. Niger is one of the 36 States in Nigeria, the state is bordered by Zamfara State in the North, Kebbi State and Benin Republic in North West, Kaduna State in           the North East, In the South it is bordered by Kogi State, South West by Kwara State and South East by Federal Capital Territory (FCT) Abuja (Source: Geography Department FUT, Minna and Ministry of Land and Survey-Niger State, Minna). Main Findings: High ability level students had higher mean (x̄) achievement score that the low ability level students Also there is a significant difference between the mean (x̄) the achievement scores of low and high ability level students. Low ability level students who were taught geography using BLM had higher mean gain achievement score than the low ability level students taught using lecture methods while the high level ability students who were taught using BLM had higher mean gain achievement score than the high ability students taught using lecture method. It was further revealed that there is no significant interaction effect of method and ability level on the mean achievement scores in Geography.


2019 ◽  
Vol 121 (2) ◽  
pp. 1-50
Author(s):  
Anthony Buttaro ◽  
Sophia Catsambis

Background Ability grouping has resurged in U.S. schools despite long-standing debates over its consequences for educational equity. Proponents argue that it is the best response to variation in academic skills because it allows teachers to customize the content and pace of instruction to students’ diverse needs. Critics answer that this practice places students in divergent educational paths that reproduce educational and social inequalities. Despite the contested nature of ability grouping, research has yet to produce reliable longitudinal evidence to evaluate critics’ claims. Objective We examine the degree to which exposure to within-class grouping for reading instruction from kindergarten to third grade is predictive of students’ reading test scores and English coursework in the middle grades. Research Design We use multilevel achievement growth models predicting average reading achievement from kindergarten to eighth grade as a function of years of exposure in low, average, or high ability groups in kindergarten through third grade and control variables relevant to each grade. We evaluate the achievement differences between students who are grouped in these ability groups for one or more years and those who were never ability grouped. We use multinomial logistic regression models to estimate the degree to which number of years in each ability group in K–3 grades predicts placements in eighth-grade English classes (below grade or honors, as opposed to regular English classes). Data We use data from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study–Kindergarten Cohort (ECLS–K), a national panel study of the 1998 U.S. kindergarten cohort sponsored by National Center for Education Statistics, U.S. Department of Education. Our sample consists of 7,800 students with data for fall of kindergarten, and spring of kindergarten and first, third, fifth, and eighth grades. Findings Compared with similar students who were ungrouped in the early grades, those in high-ability reading groups have higher test scores, whereas those in low-ability groups have lower test scores in every grade from kindergarten to the eighth grade. In addition, compared with their ungrouped counterparts, students in low-ability groups in the early grades are more likely to enroll in eighth grade English classes that are below grade level, whereas those in high-ability groups in these grades are more likely to enroll in honors eighth-grade English classes. Achievement gaps between previously grouped and ungrouped students increase with every additional year of exposure to ability grouping. Conclusions Students’ ability group placements in the early grades evolve into divergent educational paths that grow further apart with multiple years of grouping. These findings provide the first longitudinal evidence linking ability grouping to the reproduction of educational inequalities.


1968 ◽  
Vol 22 (2) ◽  
pp. 515-531 ◽  
Author(s):  
N. L. Pielstick ◽  
A. Bond Woodruff

60 children representing equally two age groups (2nd and 6th graders) and three ability groups (IQs of 92–108, 112–128, and 132+) served as Ss in studies of the effects of stimulus characteristics on exploratory behavior and the effect of novelty on immediate and delayed recall. In Exp. 1, significant effects were obtained for complexity, abstractness, and incongruity but not ambiguity of tachistoscopically presented stimulus materials. Age affected exploration of incongruous and ambiguous but not complex and abstract materials. Ability level had no significant effects. In Exp. II, exploratory time and judged intensity of exploration were influenced by the characteristics of stimulus objects presented in an array for free exploration. Novelty facilitated exploration but neither age nor ability had a significant effect. In Exp. III, both immediate and delayed recall by the younger Ss were greater for objects they identified as “novel” but recall by the older Ss was somewhat greater for “not-novel” objects. Factor analysis of all the behavioral scores derived in these experiments along with scores from a creativity test indicated that the various measures of exploratory behavior and creativity used here were sampling different aspects of behavior and no one of such measures can be assumed to be an adequate index of exploratory behavior in general.


1965 ◽  
Vol 16 (2) ◽  
pp. 659-660
Author(s):  
James G. Greeno

This study investigated the relation between performance in paired-associate learning and other verbal skills (verbal fluency to word and nonsense stimuli, recall of a word list). Ss learned two lists of word-CVC pairs. Significant positive correlations were found between fluency measures and learning of both lists. Recall scores were similarly correlated with second-list, but not first-list, learning.


2018 ◽  
Vol 9 (1) ◽  
pp. 45
Author(s):  
Asmah Asmah

Abstrak: Dilaksanakan Penelitian Tindakan Kelas dengan tujuan penelitian untuk mendeskripsikan peningkatan keterampilan berbicara untuk mengemukakan pendapat melalui metode diskusi  pada siswa kelas VI SDN 3 Barejulat  tahun 2015/2016. Metode pengumpulan data : observasi, tugas, dokumentasi, dan tes.Hasil analisis: kemampuan tinggi sebanyak 12 orang tingkat kemampuan sedang 15 orang dengan IPK 65,51 % pada kategori sedang. Sedangkan keterampilan berbicara dengan menggunakan metode diskusi dalam mengemukakan pendapat  dengan kemampuan tinggi sebanyak 25 orang, tingkat kemampuan sedang dengan jumlah siswa 2 orang dengan persentase 7,48 % dengaqn IPK pada kategori 74,00 % kategori tinggi. Jadi penggunaan metode diskusi meningkatkan keterampilan berbicara siswa  kelas VI SDN 3 Barejulat  tahun 2015/2016.Abstract:  Classroom Action Research carried out with the aim of research to describe the improvement of speech skills to express opinions through the method of discussion on students of class VI SDN 3 Barejulat year 2015/2016. Methods of data collection: observation, tasks, documentation, and tests. Result of analysis: high ability as many as 12 people medium ability level 15 people with GPA 65,51% in medium category. While speaking skill by using method of discussion in expressing opinion with high ability counted 25 people, medium ability level with number of student 2 person with percentage of 7,48% dengaqn GPA in category 74.00% high category. So the use of discussion method improves the speaking skill of grade 6 students of SDN 3 Barejulat year 2015/2016.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document