stroop effects
Recently Published Documents


TOTAL DOCUMENTS

57
(FIVE YEARS 6)

H-INDEX

19
(FIVE YEARS 1)

2021 ◽  
Vol 21 (9) ◽  
pp. 1865
Author(s):  
Aurore Zelazny ◽  
Thomas Alrik Sørensen
Keyword(s):  

Author(s):  
Yi Lin ◽  
Hongwei Ding ◽  
Yang Zhang

Purpose This study aimed to examine the Stroop effects of verbal and nonverbal cues and their relative impacts on gender differences in unisensory and multisensory emotion perception. Method Experiment 1 investigated how well 88 normal Chinese adults (43 women and 45 men) could identify emotions conveyed through face, prosody and semantics as three independent channels. Experiments 2 and 3 further explored gender differences during multisensory integration of emotion through a cross-channel (prosody-semantics) and a cross-modal (face-prosody-semantics) Stroop task, respectively, in which 78 participants (41 women and 37 men) were asked to selectively attend to one of the two or three communication channels. Results The integration of accuracy and reaction time data indicated that paralinguistic cues (i.e., face and prosody) of emotions were consistently more salient than linguistic ones (i.e., semantics) throughout the study. Additionally, women demonstrated advantages in processing all three types of emotional signals in the unisensory task, but only preserved their strengths in paralinguistic processing and showed greater Stroop effects of nonverbal cues on verbal ones during multisensory perception. Conclusions These findings demonstrate clear gender differences in verbal and nonverbal emotion perception that are modulated by sensory channels, which have important theoretical and practical implications. Supplemental Material https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.16435599


Author(s):  
Benjamin A. Parris ◽  
Nabil Hasshim ◽  
Michael Wadsley ◽  
Maria Augustinova ◽  
Ludovic Ferrand

AbstractDespite instructions to ignore the irrelevant word in the Stroop task, it robustly influences the time it takes to identify the color, leading to performance decrements (interference) or enhancements (facilitation). The present review addresses two questions: (1) What levels of processing contribute to Stroop effects; and (2) Where does attentional selection occur? The methods that are used in the Stroop literature to measure the candidate varieties of interference and facilitation are critically evaluated and the processing levels that contribute to Stroop effects are discussed. It is concluded that the literature does not provide clear evidence for a distinction between conflicting and facilitating representations at phonological, semantic and response levels (together referred to as informational conflict), because the methods do not currently permit their isolated measurement. In contrast, it is argued that the evidence for task conflict as being distinct from informational conflict is strong and, thus, that there are at least two loci of attentional selection in the Stroop task. Evidence suggests that task conflict occurs earlier, has a different developmental trajectory and is independently controlled which supports the notion of a separate mechanism of attentional selection. The modifying effects of response modes and evidence for Stroop effects at the level of response execution are also discussed. It is argued that multiple studies claiming to have distinguished response and semantic conflict have not done so unambiguously and that models of Stroop task performance need to be modified to more effectively account for the loci of Stroop effects.


Author(s):  
Masahiro Yoshihara ◽  
Mariko Nakayama ◽  
Rinus G. Verdonschot ◽  
Yasushi Hino ◽  
Stephen J. Lupker
Keyword(s):  

2019 ◽  
Vol 19 (10) ◽  
pp. 103c
Author(s):  
Amrita M Puri ◽  
Kenith V Sobel ◽  
Alxandr Kane York

2019 ◽  
Vol 148 (9) ◽  
pp. 1575-1594 ◽  
Author(s):  
Anoushiravan Zahedi ◽  
Rasha Abdel Rahman ◽  
Birgit Stürmer ◽  
Werner Sommer

2018 ◽  
Vol 22 (5) ◽  
pp. 1068-1084 ◽  
Author(s):  
JONATHAN J.D. ROBINSON ANTHONY ◽  
HENRIKE K. BLUMENFELD

Determining bilingual status has been complicated by varying interpretations of what it means to be bilingual and how to quantify bilingual experience. We examined multiple indices of language dominance (self-reported proficiency, self-reported exposure, expressive language knowledge, receptive language knowledge, and a hybrid), and whether these profiles related to performance on linguistic and cognitive tasks. Participants were administered receptive and expressive vocabulary tasks in English and Spanish, and a nonlinguistic spatial Stroop task. Analyses revealed a relation between dominance profiles and cognate and nonlinguistic Stroop effects, with somewhat different patterns emerging across measures of language dominance and variable type (continuous, categorical). Only a hybrid definition of language dominance accounted for cognate effects in the dominant language, as well as nonlinguistic spatial Stroop effects. Findings suggest that nuanced effects, such as cross-linguistic cognate effects in a dominant language and cognitive control abilities, may be particularly sensitive to operational definitions of language status.


2018 ◽  
Author(s):  
Bria Long ◽  
Mariko Moher ◽  
Susan Carey ◽  
Talia Konkle

When adults see a picture of an object, they automatically process how big the object typically is in the real world (Konkle & Oliva, 2012a). How much life experience is needed for this automatic size processing to emerge? Here, we ask whether preschoolers show this same signature of automatic size processing. We showed 3- and 4-year-olds displays with two pictures of objects and asked them to touch the picture that was smaller on the screen. Critically, the relative visual sizes of the objects could either be congruent with their relative real-world sizes (e.g., a small picture of a shoe next to a big picture of a car) or incongruent with their relative real-world sizes (e.g., a big picture of a shoe next to a small picture of a car). Across two experiments, we found that preschoolers were worse at making visual size judgments on incongruent trials, suggesting that real-world size was automatically activated and interfered with their performance. In a third experiment, we found that both 4-year-olds and adults showed similar item-pair effects (i.e., showed larger Size-Stroop effects for the pairs of items, relative to other pairs). Furthermore, the magnitude of the item-pair Stroop effects in 4-year-olds did not depend on whether they could recognize the pictured objects, suggesting that the perceptual features of these objects were sufficient to trigger the processing of real-world size information. These results indicate that, by 3–4 years of age, children automatically extract real-world size information from depicted objects.


2017 ◽  
Vol 96 ◽  
pp. 70-77 ◽  
Author(s):  
Anoushiravan Zahedi ◽  
Birgit Stuermer ◽  
Javad Hatami ◽  
Reza Rostami ◽  
Werner Sommer
Keyword(s):  

2016 ◽  
Vol 7 ◽  
Author(s):  
Eric Lorentz ◽  
Tessa McKibben ◽  
Chelsea Ekstrand ◽  
Layla Gould ◽  
Kathryn Anton ◽  
...  
Keyword(s):  

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document