Stability of Developmental Problems after School Entry of Moderately-Late Preterm and Early Preterm-Born Children

2017 ◽  
Vol 187 ◽  
pp. 73-79 ◽  
Author(s):  
Jorijn Hornman ◽  
Andrea F. de Winter ◽  
Jorien M. Kerstjens ◽  
Arend F. Bos ◽  
Sijmen A. Reijneveld
2017 ◽  
Vol 34 (1) ◽  
pp. 1-18 ◽  
Author(s):  
James Hall ◽  
Ariel Lindorff

Aims: To determine whether distinct trends can exist in children's diurnal cortisol slopes as they transition to school, and the extent to which these trends relate to preschool attendance and/or exerted effortful control. Method: A secondary analysis of the anonymised data gathered for the UK Transition to School Study was carried out. 105 children were studied over a 12-month period during transition to school at mean age 55 months. Children's diurnal cortisol slopes were measured as the difference between average salivary cortisol concentrations (SCC) sampled at waking and early evening over two days at each of three measurement time points: 4 months before, 2 weeks after, and 6 months after school entry. Children's effortful control was measured at 2 weeks after school entry using the parent-administered Child Behavior Questionnaire. Parental questionnaires recorded the duration children spent in preschool (months; days per week; hours per week), and four background characteristics: child gender, parental cohabitation, responding parent's age, and responding parent's level of education. Findings: Latent class growth analysis suggested two distinct trends in diurnal cortisol slopes during children's transition to school: 39% of children demonstrated flatter diurnal cortisol slopes. These children were likely to have spent fewer hours per week in preschool, and were likely to exert less effortful control 2 weeks after transitioning to school. These associations underscore the importance of continuity in children's daily routines as they transition to school. Implications are discussed concerning school readiness and the effectiveness of early interventions.


2011 ◽  
Vol 159 (1) ◽  
pp. 92-98 ◽  
Author(s):  
Jorien M. Kerstjens ◽  
Andrea F. de Winter ◽  
Inger F. Bocca-Tjeertes ◽  
Elisabeth M.J. ten Vergert ◽  
Sijmen A. Reijneveld ◽  
...  

2020 ◽  
Author(s):  
Hugh William Catts ◽  
Tiffany P. Hogan

Dyslexia is a significant developmental disorder that is associated with a host of negative consequences. Most states in the US have recently passed legislation requiring the diagnosis and treatment of dyslexia in school settings. Whereas this legislation brings needed attention to children with dyslexia, diagnosis and treatment are often delayed until several years after school entry. By this time, reading problems and other negative consequences are well underway. In this paper, we argue for an alternative, prevention-based approach that focuses on the early identification of children at risk for dyslexia and the provision of instruction/intervention that is matched to their needs.


PLoS ONE ◽  
2019 ◽  
Vol 14 (11) ◽  
pp. e0223690 ◽  
Author(s):  
Lilly Bogičević ◽  
Marjolein Verhoeven ◽  
Anneloes L. van Baar
Keyword(s):  

2019 ◽  
Vol 178 (4) ◽  
pp. 455-462 ◽  
Author(s):  
Koos Korsten ◽  
Maarten O. Blanken ◽  
Brigitte J. M. Buiteman ◽  
Elisabeth E. Nibbelke ◽  
Christiana A. Naaktgeboren ◽  
...  
Keyword(s):  

2015 ◽  
Vol 101 (3) ◽  
pp. 217-222 ◽  
Author(s):  
Marieke R Potijk ◽  
Andrea F de Winter ◽  
Arend F Bos ◽  
Jorien M Kerstjens ◽  
Sijmen A Reijneveld

ObjectiveTo determine the occurrence of emotional and behavioural problems (EBP) in moderate to late preterm (MLP) and full-term children with developmental delay.DesignParticipants were recruited from 13 randomly selected preventive child healthcare (PCH) centres in the Netherlands. We included 903 MLP children of 32–36 weeks’ gestation and 538 full-term controls, born between January 2002 and June 2003. Parents completed the Ages and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ) and Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL) shortly before the scheduled PCH visit at 4 years of age. Co-occurrence was defined as: ASQ total or domain score >2 SDs below the mean and a CBCL score >84th percentile on total problems, internalising (emotional) or externalising (behavioural) problems.ResultsEBP were more prevalent among MLP children with abnormal ASQ total problems scores than among full-term children, particularly regarding externalising problems (33.8% vs 23.8%). In MLP children, rates of EBP differed per developmental domain and were highest for the domains problem-solving (36.0% had externalising problems, 95% CI 24.1% to 49.9%) and personal-social skills (38.7% had internalising problems, 95% CI 26.4% to 52.8%). The risk of any type of co-occurrence was higher for MLP than for full-term children (OR 1.86; 95% CI 1.14 to 3.03). Independent risk factors for co-occurrence were male gender, low socioeconomic status and young maternal age.ConclusionsUp to 39% of 4-year-old MLP children with developmental delay also have EBP, indicating that increased awareness of EBP is warranted in MLP children with developmental delay. Further research is needed to determine whether early detection of co-occurring problems results in better long-term health.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Maggy Leblond ◽  
Sophie Parent ◽  
Natalie Castellanos-Ryan ◽  
Sonia Lupien ◽  
William D. Fraser ◽  
...  

Many studies showed a global acute increase in stress hormones at kindergarten entry, but inconsistencies among them due to small samples with varying methodologies and very few measurement points, preclude robust conclusions. The current study aimed to describe the pattern of morning cortisol concentration in children during the transition from preschool to school by examining whether we could identify certain stress responses such as stress reactivity, but also anticipatory stress and stress recovery. We further tested the robustness of this pattern across several characteristics. Participants were 384 children recruited from two cohorts of a pregnancy study, and followed across their transition from preschool to kindergarten. Five morning salivary cortisol samples were collected over time from the children: twice before school entry and three times after school entry. Although no anticipatory stress was observed two weeks before school entry, latent growth curve models showed that most children’s morning cortisol concentrations increased during the first two weeks of school, and was not associated with any sociodemographic characteristics, supporting the hypothesis that school entry is a universal social stressor. In contrast, the rate of recovery measured two months after school entry, however, revealed between children variance, which could not be explained by any specific sociodemographic characteristics. This study showed that stress reactivity at school entry is sustained for at least two weeks in most children. However, the observed variability in the rate of recovery might be associated with adaptation to the school transition and will allow further testing of functional correlates.


2012 ◽  
Vol 17 (2) ◽  
pp. 77-81 ◽  
Author(s):  
Sarah J. Kotecha ◽  
Frank D. Dunstan ◽  
Sailesh Kotecha

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document