scholarly journals Twin Studies, Molecular Genetics, Politics, and Tolerance: A Response to Beckwith and Morris

2008 ◽  
Vol 6 (4) ◽  
pp. 793-797 ◽  
Author(s):  
John R. Alford ◽  
Carolyn L. Funk ◽  
John R. Hibbing

Beckwith and Morris raise concerns about the value of twin studies for understanding the role of genetics in complex human behavior, but virtually all of their concerns have been raised and rebutted before. When it comes to the equal environments assumption (EEA), the best approach is to test for and control possible violations of the EEA on heritability estimates rather than merely rejecting all empirical evidence because of the possibility of EEA violations. In many respects, since the study of the genetic basis of complex human behaviors now includes many methods in addition to twin studies, Beckwith and Morris's critique applies more to the behavioral genetics of a quarter century ago than to today's multifaceted behavioral genetics. Twin studies establish that there is a sizeable genetic component to political orientations, thereby giving cause to look further at the nature of that role by using other methodologies, including molecular genetics. We conclude by pointing out that the normative implications of the relevance of genes to human behavior are not nearly as worrisome as Beckwith and Morris seem to believe.

Twin Research ◽  
2003 ◽  
Vol 6 (6) ◽  
pp. 486-489 ◽  
Author(s):  
Lindon Eaves ◽  
Debra Foley ◽  
Judy Silberg

AbstractArecurring criticism of the twin method for quantifying genetic and environmental components of human differences is the necessity of the so-called “equal environments assumption” (EEA) (i.e., that monozygotic and dizygotic twins experience equally correlated environments). It has been proposed to test the EEA by stratifying twin correlations by indices of the amount of shared environment. However, relevant environments may also be influenced by genetic differences. We present a model for the role of genetic factors in niche selection by twins that may account for variation in indices of the shared twin environment (e.g., contact between members of twin pairs). Simulations reveal that stratification of twin correlations by amount of contact can yield spurious evidence of large shared environmental effects in some strata and even give false indications of genotype x environment interaction. The stratification approach to testing the equal environments assumption may be misleading and the results of such tests may actually be consistent with a simpler theory of the role of genetic factors in niche selection.


2008 ◽  
Vol 6 (4) ◽  
pp. 785-791 ◽  
Author(s):  
Jon Beckwith ◽  
Corey A. Morris

Using the “classical twin method,” political scientists John Alford, Carolyn Funk, and John Hibbing conclude that political ideologies are significantly influenced by genetics, an assertion that has garnered considerable media attention. Researchers have long used human twins in attempts to assess the degree of genetic influence on various behavioral traits. Today, this methodology has been largely replaced in favor of contemporary molecular genetic techniques, and thus heritability studies have seen a diminishing role in behavioral genetic research of the twenty-first century. One important reason the twin method has been superseded is that it depends upon several questionable assumptions, the most significant of which is known as the equal environments assumption. Alford, Funk, and Hibbing argue that this crucial assumption, and thus their conclusion, holds up under empirical scrutiny. They point to several studies in support of this assumption. Here, we review the evidence presented and conclude that these attempts to test the equal environments assumption are weak, suffering significant methodological and inherent design flaws. Furthermore, much of the empirical evidence provided by these studies actually argues that, contrary to the interpretation, trait-relevant equal environments assumptions have been violated. We conclude that the equal environments assumption remains untenable, and as such, twin studies are an insufficient method for drawing meaningful conclusions regarding complex human behavior.


2003 ◽  
Vol 6 (6) ◽  
pp. 486-489 ◽  
Author(s):  
Lindon Eaves ◽  
Debra Foley ◽  
Judy Silberg

2017 ◽  
Vol 15 (1) ◽  
pp. 93-105
Author(s):  
Katarzyna Ślebarska

Abstract The proactive dimension of human behavior is rooted in one’s need to create and control the environment. Individuals prefer to do things actively and creatively rather than being counteractive. The dynamics of the job market demand that individuals are increasingly independent and proactive, can easily adapt to change, and create their own future. This way of understanding a newcomer’s activity corresponds to proactive coping. The main goal of this study was to investigate the role of proactive coping of workers in a new workplace and in job adaptation outcomes, namely well-being. Data was collected from newly employed workers (N = 172) who agreed to participate in the study within a longitudinal evaluation design (one pre-test and a double post-test) during their first six months in a new workplace. Overall, the study demonstrates that proactive coping improves the adaptation of new employees, costing them less emotionally as they adjust to their new workplace. Additionally, the employees’ pre-entry experience (previously unemployed vs previously employed) moderated the relation between the analyzed variables.


2020 ◽  
Author(s):  
Sieun An ◽  
Viraj Dhiren Malani ◽  
Aanchal Setia

Superstitions and the placebo effect have been found to influence human behavior. The present study aimed to determine whether there is a relationship between superstition and the placebo effect, and whether it affects human cognition and behavior. We hypothesized that more superstitious people would be more prone to the placebo effect and that it would improve their performance on cognitive tasks. We employed a fully between participants design, with placebo and control conditions and superstition as a constructive measure. The results showed that, in the placebo condition, more superstitious people memorized more words than less superstitious people. However, in the control condition, less superstitious people memorized more words than more superstitious people. Overall, the findings supported our hypothesis. The findings of the study are important, as they draw a link between the placebo effect and superstition, and further show that these two elements impact human performance in cognitive ability tasks.


2020 ◽  
Author(s):  
Edwin S. Dalmaijer

SummaryFor a century [1,2], studies of monozygotic and dizygotic twins have yielded estimates of trait heritability. The clever logic behind them is that while both types of twins share environments, their genetic overlap is different. Hence, larger trait correlations between monozygotic compared to dizygotic twins indicate heritability (nature), whereas similar correlations indicate shared environmental influences (nurture), and low correlations indicate shaping through non-shared environments (external influences and measurement error). While many have written on the assumptions that both types of twins share equal environments [3–5], and that parental genetics and environment are independent [6,7]; fewer have put their data where their mouth is. Here, the impacts of unmet assumptions were investigated using a generative mixture model of twin phenotypes. The results indicated that violations of the equal environments assumption yielded large overestimations of heritability and underestimations of shared environmental influences. On the other hand, when parental genetics shaped twins’ shared environments, only minor non-linear biases against heritability emerged. Finally, realistic levels of measurement error uniformly depressed estimates for genetic and shared environmental factors. In sum, twin studies are particularly susceptible to overestimation of genetic and non-shared environmental influences. This bias could explain why some traits, such as attitudes towards property taxes [8], show suspiciously high heritability without a biologically plausible mechanism. Particularly in the context of traits with convincing mechanisms of cultural transmission [9–11] and complex gene-environment interactions [6], researchers should not allow biases in twin studies to overestimate heritability.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document