Binocular Rivalry in Half-Occluded Regions of Coloured Random-Dot Stereograms

Perception ◽  
1996 ◽  
Vol 25 (1_suppl) ◽  
pp. 154-154
Author(s):  
M Suzuki

In normal perception, binocular rivalry does not arise in half-occluded regions. However, when coloured random-dot stereograms (RDSs) were observed, binocular rivalry arose in half-occluded regions. In this study, binocular rivalry in half-occluded regions was studied with coloured RDSs. Coloured RDSs consisted of a central square-shaped region, a background, half-occluded regions, and a probe. The central square-shaped region was coloured white, and was presented in either crossed or uncrossed disparity. The background was coloured yellow. The half-occluded regions were coloured either white, yellow, or blue. The probe was coloured red, and was presented in either of the half-occluded regions. Subjects judged the disappearance of the probe and stereopsis. When the half-occluded regions were coloured the same as the front plane, the probe and stereopsis both disappeared. On the other hand, when the half-occluded regions were coloured the same as the back plane or differently from both planes only the probe disappeared while stereopsis did not disappear. Also, disappearance of the probe decreased more when the half-occluded regions were coloured the same as the back plane, than when the half-occluded regions were coloured differently from both planes. These results suggest that half-occluded regions escape binocular rivalry for several reasons, including ecological validity in 3-D representation, and that half-occluded regions are suppressed interocularly when a static stereogram is observed statically.

2018 ◽  
Author(s):  
Shui'Er Han ◽  
Raphael Lukaszewski ◽  
David Alais

Continuous flash suppression (CFS) is a technique in which presenting one eyewith a dynamic Mondrian sequence prevents a low-contrast target in the other eye frombeing perceived for many seconds. Frequently used to study unconscious visualprocessing, CFS bears many similarities with binocular rivalry (BR), another populardichoptic stimulation technique. It is therefore puzzling that the effect of mask size andcontrast seem to differ between CFS and BR. To resolve this discrepancy, we conducted asystematic investigation on the effects of mask size and contrast in CFS. Also, buildingon findings from BR, we asked if the collinearity of the contours in the Mondrian maskerplay a role in CFS suppression. Our results showed a robust effect of mask contrast onsuppression durations, and an effect of mask size that depended on collinearity.Specifically, higher mask contrasts produced longer suppression regardless of collinearityand mask size. Mask size, on the other hand, had little effect on suppression whencollinearity was low and it weakened suppression when collinearity is high. Theseobservations parallel prior findings in BR, further substantiating the close link betweenthe two paradigms and demonstrating the usefulness of a shared explanatory frameworkdescribing both phenomena.


1994 ◽  
Vol 11 (4) ◽  
pp. 833-837 ◽  
Author(s):  
K. Kawano ◽  
Y. Inoue ◽  
A. Takemura ◽  
F. A. Miles

AbstractOcular following responses induced by brief movements of the visual scene were examined in monkeys for their dependence on disparity in the peripheral field. A random dot pattern was projected onto a tangent screen and partitioned into central and peripheral regions. Test stimuli were velocity steps applied in the central region, while stimuli in the periphery were stationary. The visual images in the central region were seen always in the plane of the screen, while stimuli in the periphery could be seen in front, behind, or in the plane of the screen (achieved by a system of polarizing filters). Initial ocular following responses were larger when the peripheral stimuli were presented with an uncrossed disparity than without disparity. On the other hand, responses were smaller when the peripheral stimuli were presented with crossed disparity (<5.0 deg) than without disparity. The result is consistent with the idea that ocular following responses are dependent on the perceived viewing distance.


Perception ◽  
1993 ◽  
Vol 22 (12) ◽  
pp. 1403-1413 ◽  
Author(s):  
Akira Ishigushi ◽  
Jeremy M Wolfe

When a real or subjective contour is superimposed on a repetitive texture or ‘wallpaper pattern’, the apparent depth of the elements of the wallpaper pattern may be influenced by the contour. When the contour has crossed disparity, the elements inside the contour are seen floating in the plane of the contour. This is called ‘stereoscopic capture’. On the other hand, when the contour has uncrossed disparity a different, somewhat unstable, percept is seen. The wallpaper elements are either seen to form a transparent surface floating in front of the contour, or all of the elements inside and outside of the contour are seen to lie in the uncrossed plane of the inducing contour. We suggest that the asymmetry between crossed and uncrossed stereo capture is a by-product of the different roles played by crossed and uncrossed disparity in stereoscopic surface construction: specifically, crossed and, perhaps, zero disparity spreads one-directionally into a figure from a contour whereas uncrossed disparity spreads out from a figure in all directions. These points are illustrated with a series of demonstrations.


1999 ◽  
Vol 173 ◽  
pp. 249-254
Author(s):  
A.M. Silva ◽  
R.D. Miró

AbstractWe have developed a model for theH2OandOHevolution in a comet outburst, assuming that together with the gas, a distribution of icy grains is ejected. With an initial mass of icy grains of 108kg released, theH2OandOHproductions are increased up to a factor two, and the growth curves change drastically in the first two days. The model is applied to eruptions detected in theOHradio monitorings and fits well with the slow variations in the flux. On the other hand, several events of short duration appear, consisting of a sudden rise ofOHflux, followed by a sudden decay on the second day. These apparent short bursts are frequently found as precursors of a more durable eruption. We suggest that both of them are part of a unique eruption, and that the sudden decay is due to collisions that de-excite theOHmaser, when it reaches the Cometopause region located at 1.35 × 105kmfrom the nucleus.


Author(s):  
A. V. Crewe

We have become accustomed to differentiating between the scanning microscope and the conventional transmission microscope according to the resolving power which the two instruments offer. The conventional microscope is capable of a point resolution of a few angstroms and line resolutions of periodic objects of about 1Å. On the other hand, the scanning microscope, in its normal form, is not ordinarily capable of a point resolution better than 100Å. Upon examining reasons for the 100Å limitation, it becomes clear that this is based more on tradition than reason, and in particular, it is a condition imposed upon the microscope by adherence to thermal sources of electrons.


Author(s):  
K.H. Westmacott

Life beyond 1MeV – like life after 40 – is not too different unless one takes advantage of past experience and is receptive to new opportunities. At first glance, the returns on performing electron microscopy at voltages greater than 1MeV diminish rather rapidly as the curves which describe the well-known advantages of HVEM often tend towards saturation. However, in a country with a significant HVEM capability, a good case can be made for investing in instruments with a range of maximum accelerating voltages. In this regard, the 1.5MeV KRATOS HVEM being installed in Berkeley will complement the other 650KeV, 1MeV, and 1.2MeV instruments currently operating in the U.S. One other consideration suggests that 1.5MeV is an optimum voltage machine – Its additional advantages may be purchased for not much more than a 1MeV instrument. On the other hand, the 3MeV HVEM's which seem to be operated at 2MeV maximum, are much more expensive.


2005 ◽  
Vol 19 (3) ◽  
pp. 129-132 ◽  
Author(s):  
Reimer Kornmann

Summary: My comment is basically restricted to the situation in which less-able students find themselves and refers only to literature in German. From this point of view I am basically able to confirm Marsh's results. It must, however, be said that with less-able pupils the opposite effect can be found: Levels of self-esteem in these pupils are raised, at least temporarily, by separate instruction, academic performance however drops; combined instruction, on the other hand, leads to improved academic performance, while levels of self-esteem drop. Apparently, the positive self-image of less-able pupils who receive separate instruction does not bring about the potential enhancement of academic performance one might expect from high-ability pupils receiving separate instruction. To resolve the dilemma, it is proposed that individual progress in learning be accentuated, and that comparisons with others be dispensed with. This fosters a self-image that can in equal measure be realistic and optimistic.


Author(s):  
Stefan Krause ◽  
Markus Appel

Abstract. Two experiments examined the influence of stories on recipients’ self-perceptions. Extending prior theory and research, our focus was on assimilation effects (i.e., changes in self-perception in line with a protagonist’s traits) as well as on contrast effects (i.e., changes in self-perception in contrast to a protagonist’s traits). In Experiment 1 ( N = 113), implicit and explicit conscientiousness were assessed after participants read a story about either a diligent or a negligent student. Moderation analyses showed that highly transported participants and participants with lower counterarguing scores assimilate the depicted traits of a story protagonist, as indicated by explicit, self-reported conscientiousness ratings. Participants, who were more critical toward a story (i.e., higher counterarguing) and with a lower degree of transportation, showed contrast effects. In Experiment 2 ( N = 103), we manipulated transportation and counterarguing, but we could not identify an effect on participants’ self-ascribed level of conscientiousness. A mini meta-analysis across both experiments revealed significant positive overall associations between transportation and counterarguing on the one hand and story-consistent self-reported conscientiousness on the other hand.


2019 ◽  
Vol 50 (2) ◽  
pp. 80-93
Author(s):  
Jort de Vreeze ◽  
Christina Matschke

Abstract. Not all group memberships are self-chosen. The current research examines whether assignments to non-preferred groups influence our relationship with the group and our preference for information about the ingroup. It was expected and found that, when people are assigned to non-preferred groups, they perceive the group as different to the self, experience negative emotions about the assignment and in turn disidentify with the group. On the other hand, when people are assigned to preferred groups, they perceive the group as similar to the self, experience positive emotions about the assignment and in turn identify with the group. Finally, disidentification increases a preference for negative information about the ingroup.


1961 ◽  
Vol 6 (6) ◽  
pp. 222-223
Author(s):  
Edwin G. Boring
Keyword(s):  

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document