scholarly journals What causes the word gap? Financial concerns may systematically suppress child‐directed speech

2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Monica E. Ellwood‐Lowe ◽  
Ruthe Foushee ◽  
Mahesh Srinivasan
Keyword(s):  
2016 ◽  
Vol 10 (2) ◽  
pp. 134-139 ◽  
Author(s):  
Annemarie H. Hindman ◽  
Barbara A. Wasik ◽  
Emily K. Snell
Keyword(s):  

PEDIATRICS ◽  
2018 ◽  
Vol 142 (6) ◽  
pp. e20181992 ◽  
Author(s):  
Mical Raz ◽  
Barbara R. Beatty
Keyword(s):  

2018 ◽  
Vol 90 (3) ◽  
pp. 985-992 ◽  
Author(s):  
Roberta Michnick Golinkoff ◽  
Erika Hoff ◽  
Meredith L. Rowe ◽  
Catherine S. Tamis‐LeMonda ◽  
Kathy Hirsh‐Pasek
Keyword(s):  

2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
John Coleman Flournoy

Preregistration establishes transparent and verifiable links between hypotheses, statistical tests, and error-control of decisions. In this comment, I critique both the preregistration of, and the presentation of preregistered results from two studies investigating scarcity as a potential cause of the word gap. The primary goal is to illustrate how and why preregistration should guide the way results are framed, and how this might influence conclusions drawn.


2020 ◽  
Vol 36 (1) ◽  
pp. 33-57 ◽  
Author(s):  
Melissa Baralt ◽  
Ashley Darcy Mahoney ◽  
Natalie Brito

The early language environments of low-income Hispanic children can be negatively affected when their Spanish-speaking caregivers face racism, assimilation pressure, and/or misinformed advice based on English-only ideologies. This article reports on the design and efficacy of Háblame Bebé, a language-promoting phone application that encourages low-income Hispanic mothers to talk more to their children in their native Spanish with the goals of (1) improving their children’s early language environment, (2) promoting bilingualism, and (3) monitoring developmental milestones. The app was designed and tested across three phases as mandated by the US HRSA Bridging the Word Gap Challenge. In Phase I, we developed a curriculum that promotes high-quality language interactions in Spanish and designed the app components. In Phase II, we tested the app with 20 Hispanic mothers (half high school-educated, half college-educated) in a pretest–posttest design in which we examined their language interactions before and after two months of using the app. Preliminary results indicated that mother–child verbal interactions increased, but not always in their native Spanish, and the difference was not statistically significant. Focus group data revealed that many of the mothers had experienced linguistic racism and that tropes surrounding Spanish-speaking identity in the USA needed to be explicitly addressed within the intervention. In Phase III, a sociolinguistic pride component was added and the app was again tested with 12 additional Hispanic mothers (all high school-educated only). This time, a statistically significant increase in mother–child verbal interactions was found. Mothers also reported feeling prouder to use Spanish with their children. These results suggest that Háblame Bebé may be a viable means to reach low-income Hispanic caregivers who face obstacles in accessing health information and/or home-visiting programs for their children.


2020 ◽  
Author(s):  
Mary E Brushe ◽  
John W. Lynch ◽  
Sheena Reilly ◽  
Edward Melhuish ◽  
Sally A. Brinkman

Abstract Background: There is evidence that parents from more socioeconomically disadvantaged backgrounds engage in fewer verbal interactions with their child than more advantaged parents. This leads to the so-called, ‘30 million-word gap’. We used improved technology to investigate the number of words children hear in their first year of life. Methods: Mothers were recruited into a five-year prospective cohort study and categorized into either high or low maternal education groups. Data was derived from the first two waves of the study, when the children were six and twelve months old. At both waves, children were involved in day-long audio recordings using the Language Environment Analysis software that provided automatic counts of adult words spoken to the child, child vocalizations and conversational turns. Descriptive results are presented by maternal education groups. Results: There was large variation within each maternal education group, with the number of adult words spoken to the child ranging from 2,958 to 39,583 at six months and 4,389 to 45,849 at twelve months. There were no meaningful differences between adult words, child vocalizations or conversational turns across maternal education groups at either wave of data collection. Conclusions: These results show that a word gap related to maternal education is not apparent up to twelve months of age. The large variability among both maternal education groups suggests that universal interventions that encourage all parents to talk more to their child may be more appropriate than interventions targeted towards disadvantaged families during the first year of life.


Author(s):  
Charles R. Greenwood ◽  
Judith J. Carta ◽  
Alana G. Schnitz ◽  
Dale Walker ◽  
Dola Gabriel ◽  
...  

2020 ◽  
Author(s):  
Mary E Brushe ◽  
John W. Lynch ◽  
Sheena Reilly ◽  
Edward Melhuish ◽  
Sally A. Brinkman

Abstract Background: There is evidence that parents from more socioeconomically disadvantaged backgrounds engage in fewer verbal interactions with their child than more advantaged parents. This leads to the so-called, ‘30 million-word gap’. Methods: We used improved technology to investigate the number of words children hear in their first year of life. Mothers were recruited into a five-year prospective cohort study and categorized into either high or low maternal education groups. Data was derived from the first two waves of the study, when the children were six and twelve months old. At both waves, children were involved in day-long audio recordings using the Language Environment Analysis software that provided automatic counts of adult words spoken to the child, child vocalizations and conversational turns. Descriptive results are presented by maternal education groups. Results: There was large variation within each maternal education group, with the number of adult words spoken to the child ranging from 2,958 to 39,583 at six months and 4,389 to 45,849 at twelve months. There were no meaningful differences between adult words, child vocalizations or conversational turns across maternal education groups at either wave of data collection. Conclusions: These results show that a word gap related to maternal education is not apparent up to twelve months of age. The large variability among both maternal education groups suggests that universal interventions that encourage all parents to talk more to their child may be more appropriate than interventions targeted towards disadvantaged families during the first year of life.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document