The behaviour of mature male sperm whales on the Galápagos Islands breeding grounds

1993 ◽  
Vol 71 (4) ◽  
pp. 689-699 ◽  
Author(s):  
Hal Whitehead

The behaviour of mature male sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus) was observed off the Galápagos Islands between 1985 and 1991. The abundance of males peaked in April and May at 3% of the population. Only 1 of the 18 photographically identified males was sighted off the Galápagos in two different years. In their residency periods off the Galápagos and their lack of preferred ranges, and in many aspects of their behaviour, males were similar to females. However, unlike females, males were sometimes seen alone and never performed any aerial behaviour. Although they were occasionally seen or heard together, there are indications that males avoided one another, perhaps by listening for the "slow click" vocalizations made by males approximately 75% of the time. One incidence of possible aggression between males was observed, and many males possessed parallel scars on their heads, presumably made during aggressive encounters. Males moved between groups of females, spending very approximately 8 h with each group. Groups of females were often revisited by particular males over periods of a few days but never over more than 1 week. Males showed no obvious preference for particular groups, although a few close associations between individual males and individual females were noticed over intervals of a few days. When males were present, females showed increased rates of making spyhops and sideflukes (indicating manoeuvring) and codas (a communicative vocalization). Copulation was not observed. The evidence is consistent with males' maximizing their expected reproductive success by roving between groups of females.

1988 ◽  
Vol 66 (9) ◽  
pp. 1931-1937 ◽  
Author(s):  
Linda S. Weilgart ◽  
Hal Whitehead

Groups of sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus) were tracked acoustically off the Galapagos Islands between February and April 1985. In total, 716 h were spent in visual or acoustic contact with the whales, during which time vocalizations were recorded for 5 min/h. Distinctive loud, ringing clicks, called "slow clicks," were highly correlated with the presence of mature male sperm whales. Slow clicks were distinguished from usual clicks by their slower repetition rate or interclick interval, their longer duration, and, usually the presence of intensity peaks at about 1.8 and 2.8 kHz. Between clicks of individual males (identified by photographs of natural markings), there were differences both in interclick intervals and in the pattern of emphasized frequencies. These differences, however, were not distinct enough to allow us to reliably distinguish one male's clicks from those of another. We hypothesize that slow clicks may be a sign of a mature or maturing male and may inform other sperm whales on the breeding grounds of its competitive ability and maturity.


1987 ◽  
Vol 65 (4) ◽  
pp. 913-919 ◽  
Author(s):  
Hal Whitehead ◽  
Tom Arnbom

Between February and April 1985 sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus) were tracked visually and acoustically in the waters west of the Galapagos Islands. Individuals were identified and measured photographically. Using statistical criteria the female and immature whales encountered were clustered into 13 groups with closed membership during the study period. These groups had a median estimated population of 19.5 animals each, and associated with one another at different times. The groups showed no significant immigration into or emigration from the study area during the study. Seven large males were identified. These associated with each other and the groups of females and immatures for periods lasting approximately 6 h. Individual males were found with different groups of females and immatures at different times. A simple model suggests that a male employing a "searching" strategy should encounter more oestrous females than the traditionally accepted "harem" holder when the interval between encountering groups of females is less than the duration of the females' oestrus. Neither the groups of females nor the mature males appeared to possess territories or preferred ranges within the study area. The number of large males encountered was considerably less than that expected if all large males were present on the breeding grounds, suggesting that males may not breed every year.


1992 ◽  
Vol 49 (1) ◽  
pp. 78-84 ◽  
Author(s):  
Hal Whitehead ◽  
Susan Waters ◽  
Thomas Lyrholm

The structure of the population of female and immature sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus) in the region of the Galápagos Islands was studied using individual photographic identifications of 1285 animals collected between 1985 and 1989. Population parameters were estimated using a maximum-likelihood mark–recapture estimate permitting emigration from the study area in which identifications are collected and then reimmigration back into it. Because permanent associations among whales violated assumptions of independence, confidence intervals for the estimates were constructed using Monte-Carlo population simulation. The analysis suggested that there is a population of very approximately 200 whales in the study area around the islands at any time. These were part of a larger population numbering between 2600 and 5300 individuals (95% confidence interval). An average of 39–94% (95% confidence interval) of the whales left the study area in any month, with a similar number immigrating.


1993 ◽  
Vol 71 (4) ◽  
pp. 744-752 ◽  
Author(s):  
Linda Weilgart ◽  
Hal Whitehead

To gain insight into the function of sperm whale vocalizations known as codas (short, patterned series of clicks), sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus) were tracked continuously for periods of days totalling months off the Galápagos Islands, Ecuador, and vocalizations were tape recorded systematically. In total, 1333 codas were classified according to their temporal pattern and the number of clicks they contained. Codas were found to be temporally very clustered, and could be categorized into 23 fairly discrete types. Sequential analysis of codas revealed that they overlapped one another according to type in a nonrandom way, and that type 5 tended to initiate coda exchanges. "Regular" coda types (with evenly spaced clicks) tended to occur with other regular coda types and "irregular" coda types (with one or two delayed final clicks) were heard with other irregular coda types. Codas may function principally as a means of communication, to maintain social cohesion within stable groups of females following periods of dispersion during foraging.


1989 ◽  
Vol 67 (4) ◽  
pp. 839-846 ◽  
Author(s):  
Vassili Papastavrou ◽  
Sean C. Smith ◽  
Hal Whitehead

Sperm whales, Physeter macrocephalus, were tracked by means of a recording depth sounder in the waters off the Galapagos Islands. At depths of less than 300 m the whales generally dived nearly vertically at 60–100 m/min. At greater depths their descents were usually slower. Between February and April 1985, they dived to about 420 m, which is approximately the depth of the oxygen minimum. In 1987, a year of warmer water temperatures, they usually dived about 70 m shallower. There was no apparent diurnal variation in dive depths. None of the whales tracked dived to the ocean floor. Whales dived for about 40 min, followed by 10 min at the surface. Sperm whales usually started to make regular clicks when 150–300 m deep. Young calves appeared not to make prolonged deep dives. Our results are generally consistent with other direct information on the diving behaviour of relatively undisturbed sperm whales, but often conflict with results obtained using sonar for sperm whales being chased by whale catchers.


1992 ◽  
Vol 70 (11) ◽  
pp. 2145-2149 ◽  
Author(s):  
Hal Whitehead ◽  
Benjamin Kahn

The social structure of female sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus) was examined from data collected during studies of identified individuals off the Galápagos Islands in 1985, 1987, and 1989, off the Seychelles Islands in 1990, and off mainland Ecuador in 1991. Three levels of social organization were examined in the Galápagos Islands studies. "Units," permanent associations between individuals, showed no significant deviation from a mean size of 13 individuals. "Groups," temporary associations between units lasting a few days, had a mean size of 24 individuals. Group size was lower in 1987 (an "El Niño" year) than in 1985 or 1989, but this difference was not statistically significant. "Aggregations" of groups in the same general area lasting periods of hours were significantly larger in 1985 (mean of 77 individuals) than in 1987 (mean of 47 individuals). There are significant differences between areas in aggregation sizes but not in group sizes. Groups appear to aggregate frequently off the Galápagos Islands, occasionally off mainland Ecuador, and very rarely off the Seychelles Islands. These temporal and geographic differences in sizes of groups and aggregations in sperm whales are probably caused by variations in the distribution and abundance of their food, but responses to recent exploitation may also be a factor.


1992 ◽  
Vol 70 (5) ◽  
pp. 912-918 ◽  
Author(s):  
Hal Whitehead ◽  
Sebastian Brennan ◽  
David Grover

Studies of mature male sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus) were carried out on the Scotian Shelf during the summers of 1986, 1988, 1989, and 1990. Sperm whales were found principally along the edge of the Shelf, although there were some encounters in water less than 100 m deep. Within the Gully, a prominent submarine canyon, sperm whales showed a consistent pattern of distribution between years, distinct from that of northern bottlenose whales (Hyperoodon ampullatus). Some male sperm whales were individually identified in the Gully after periods of 2–47 days, and in one case, after 2 years. The male sperm whales showed little evidence of social organization and rarely performed aerial behaviour. However, their diving behaviour was similar to that observed from female sperm whales in the tropics. On one occasion a group of presumed female and immature sperm whales entered the Gully, attracting the attention of resident mature males.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document