Retinal image motion alone does not control disconjugate postsaccadic eye drift

1990 ◽  
Vol 63 (5) ◽  
pp. 999-1009 ◽  
Author(s):  
Z. Kapoula ◽  
L. M. Optican ◽  
D. A. Robinson

1. In these experiments, postsaccadic ocular drift was induced by postsaccadic motion of the visual scene. In the most important case, the scene was moved in one eye but not the other. Six human subjects viewed the interior of a full-field hemisphere filled with a random-dot pattern. During training, eye movements were recorded by the electrooculogram. A computer detected the end of every saccade and immediately moved the pattern horizontally in the same or, in different experiments, in the opposite direction as the saccade. The pattern motion was exponential with an amplitude of 25% of the size of the antecedent saccade and a time constant of 50 ms. Before and after 3-4 h of such training, movements of both eyes were measured simultaneously by the eye coil-magnetic field method while subjects looked between stationary targets for calibration, explored the visual pattern with saccades, or made saccades in the dark to measure the effects of adaptation on postsaccadic ocular drift. The amplitude of this drift was expressed as a percentage of the size of the antecedent saccade. 2. In monocular experiments, subjects viewed the random-dot pattern with one eye. The other eye was patched. With two subjects, the pattern drifted backward in the direction opposite to the saccade; with the third, it drifted onward. The induced ocular drift was exponential, always in the direction to reduce retinal image motion, had zero latency, and persisted in the dark. After training, drift in the dark changed by 6.7% in agreement with our prior study with binocular vision, which produced a change of 6.0%. 3. In a dichoptic arrangement, one eye regarded the moveable random-dot pattern; the other, through mirrors, saw a different random-dot pattern (with similar spacing, contrast, and distance) that was stationary. These visual patterns were not fuseable and did not evoke subjective diplopia. In this case, the induced change in postsaccadic drift in the same three subjects was only 4.8%. In all cases the changes in postsaccadic drift were conjugate--they obeyed Hering's law. 4. Normal human saccades are characterized by essentially no postsaccadic drift in the abducting eye and a pronounced onward drift (approximately 4%) in the adducting eye. After training, this abduction-adduction asymmetry was preserved in the light and dark with monocular or dichoptic viewing, indicating again that all adaptive changes were conjugate. 5. When the subjects viewed the adapting stimulus after training, the zero-latency, postsaccadic drift always increased from levels in the dark.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)

1993 ◽  
Vol 69 (4) ◽  
pp. 1031-1043 ◽  
Author(s):  
Z. Kapoula ◽  
D. A. Robinson ◽  
L. M. Optican

1. It has been previously shown that, if a visual pattern is transiently moved just after every saccade, it is possible to induce horizontal, postsaccadic, ocular drift after horizontal saccades that persists in the dark. In this study we show that horizontal ocular drift can also be created after vertical saccades. Five human subjects viewed binocularly the interior of a full-field hemisphere filled with a random-dot pattern. They were encouraged to make frequent vertical saccades. During training, eye movements were recorded by the electrooculogram. A computer detected the end of every saccade and immediately moved the pattern to the left after up saccades and right after down saccades. The motion was exponential, its amplitude was 25% of the vertical component of the antecedent saccade, its time constant was 50 ms. Before and after 2-3 h of training, movements of both eyes were measured by the eye-coil/magnetic-field method while subjects were instructed to make vertical saccades in the dark, in the presence of the movable adapting pattern, and between stationary targets for calibration. 2. After training (approximately 20,000 saccades) all subjects developed a zero-latency, exponential ocular drift to the left after up saccades and to the right after down saccades. The amplitude of the horizontal drift, expressed as a percentage of the vertical component of the preceding saccade, was 2.7% in the dark. This rose to 10.2% in the presence of the movable adapting stimulus. The latter rise is not due to visual following systems but to a zero-latency increase in initial drift velocity. 3. The horizontal drifts were usually unequal between the two eyes, indicating the presence of disconjugate movements. We measured intrasaccadic disconjugate horizontal movements of all subjects. In agreement with studies by others of saccades in the light, we measured a divergence during up saccades (1.3 degrees) and a convergence for down (0.4 degrees), but in this case for spontaneous saccades in the dark. After training, these values increased for saccades in the dark but decreased in the light in the presence of the adapting stimulus. These changes were largely idiosyncratic and statistically significant in only a few subjects. 4. The cross-axis postsaccadic drifts were separated into their conjugate and disconjugate components. The disconjugate components were small and idiosyncratic, and the means were small for saccades in the dark. The only consistent trend was in the presence of the adapting stimulus where up saccades were often followed by convergence.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)


1989 ◽  
Vol 61 (5) ◽  
pp. 879-891 ◽  
Author(s):  
Z. Kapoula ◽  
L. M. Optican ◽  
D. A. Robinson

1. Five human subjects viewed binocularly the interior of a full-field hemisphere filled with a random-dot pattern. During training, eye movements were recorded by the electrooculogram. A computer detected the end of every saccade and immediately moved the pattern horizontally either in the same or, in different experiments, the opposite direction as the saccade. The motion was exponential, its amplitude was 25% of the horizontal component of the antecedent saccade, and its time constant was either 25, 50, or 100 ms in different experiments. Before and after 2-3 h of this experience, movements of both eyes were measured simultaneously by the eye-coil/magnetic-field method while subjects made saccades across the moveable pattern, looked between stationary targets, or made saccades in the dark, to see the effect of such adaptation on postsaccadic eye movements. 2. After 2-3 h (10,000-20,000 saccades) subjects developed a zero-latency, postsaccadic, ocular drift in the dark in the direction of the pattern motion. Three subjects were trained to backward drift, two to onward drift. Drift amplitude in the dark changed by 6% of the saccade size (range: 2-11%). The drift was exponential with an overall time constant of 108 ms. 3. After training, while viewing the adapting pattern motion, the change in the amplitude of the zero-latency drift was approximately 10% (range: 6.5-14%). 4. Increasing the time constant of the pattern motion produced significant increases in the time constant of the ocular drift. 5. The incidence of dynamic overshoot (a tiny, backward saccade immediately following a main saccade) was idiosyncratic and went up in some subjects and down in others with adaptation. These changes did not seem related to modifications of postsaccadic drift. 6. Normal human saccades are characterized by essentially no postsaccadic drift in the abducting eye and a pronounced onward drift (approximately 4%) in the adducting eye. This adduction-adduction asymmetry is largely preserved through adaptation. Thus the changes in drift were conjugate and conformed to Hering's law of equal (change of) innervation. 7. These results agree with those previously demonstrated in the monkey and can similarly be explained by parametric changes in the pulse, slide, and step of normal saccadic innervation.


2000 ◽  
Vol 78 (2) ◽  
pp. 131-142 ◽  
Author(s):  
James W. Ness ◽  
Harry Zwick ◽  
Bruce E. Stuck ◽  
David J. Lurid ◽  
Brian J. Lurid ◽  
...  

1981 ◽  
Vol 374 (1 Vestibular an) ◽  
pp. 312-329 ◽  
Author(s):  
H. Collewijn ◽  
A. J. Martins ◽  
R. M. Steinman

Perception ◽  
1996 ◽  
Vol 25 (7) ◽  
pp. 797-814 ◽  
Author(s):  
Michiteru Kitazaki ◽  
Shinsuke Shimojo

The generic-view principle (GVP) states that given a 2-D image the visual system interprets it as a generic view of a 3-D scene when possible. The GVP was applied to 3-D-motion perception to show how the visual system decomposes retinal image motion into three components of 3-D motion: stretch/shrinkage, rotation, and translation. First, the optical process of retinal image motion was analyzed, and predictions were made based on the GVP in the inverse-optical process. Then experiments were conducted in which the subject judged perception of stretch/shrinkage, rotation in depth, and translation in depth for a moving bar stimulus. Retinal-image parameters—2-D stretch/shrinkage, 2-D rotation, and 2-D translation—were manipulated categorically and exhaustively. The results were highly consistent with the predictions. The GVP seems to offer a broad and general framework for understanding the ambiguity-solving process in motion perception. Its relationship to other constraints such as that of rigidity is discussed.


1984 ◽  
Vol 247 (2) ◽  
pp. E190-E197
Author(s):  
L. A. Leiter ◽  
M. Grose ◽  
J. F. Yale ◽  
E. B. Marliss

Catecholamines have multiple metabolic and fluid-electrolyte as well as cardiovascular effects, and their levels in plasma respond to alterations in nutrient and sodium intakes. Plasma norepinephrine, epinephrine, and dopamine were measured in 12 obese nondiabetic subjects before and after 400 kcal/day diets of either protein or glucose, followed by total fasting, and then by hypocaloric refeeding, each for 14-day periods. Measurements were made in the supine and upright posture and during and after 6-10 min of exhaustive exercise at 80% maximal VO2. Sodium intake varied with the nutrients ingested, being markedly decreased with the 400 kcal/day diets and fasting. Norepinephrine levels were higher in fasting than base line while subjects were lying or standing and after recovery from exercise. Those of epinephrine were elevated on standing. No differences were found after the 400 kcal/day diets, with protein and glucose yielding equivalent results. In contrast, refeeding was associated with lower norepinephrine levels than all other diets while subjects were lying or standing and after recovery from exercise. Peak levels of norepinephrine and epinephrine with exercise were indistinguishable among diets. Fasting was associated with lower systolic and diastolic blood pressures and lower responses to standing and exercise than base line, whereas upright heart rate was greater and that during exercise less than base line. Significant though less extensive changes occurred with the other diets. Thus both electrolyte status and nutrient intakes interact to determine net catecholamine responses, and the former seem to override the latter.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)


PLoS ONE ◽  
2013 ◽  
Vol 8 (1) ◽  
pp. e54549 ◽  
Author(s):  
Yusuke Tani ◽  
Keisuke Araki ◽  
Takehiro Nagai ◽  
Kowa Koida ◽  
Shigeki Nakauchi ◽  
...  

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document